© Distribution of this video is restricted by its owner
Transcript ×
Auto highlight
Font-size
00:02 This is lecture 11 of Neuroscience. we ended last time when we started

00:07 about the different ways that neurons have integrate the signals and different ways that

00:13 turn neurons may want to impact the depolarization of the synoptic neuron. So

00:20 talked about spatial summation where multiple synopsis target the same dendrites and will produce

00:27 potentials at the same time. And have the highest ST response when you

00:33 across the space at the same And another way is temporal summation.

00:38 the same axon produces a train or number of action potentials very closely dispersed

00:45 space and time. And that also for the E ESB to grow,

00:53 it doesn't reach the same aptitude as would have in the cases of spatial

01:00 . Another thing that happens that we is that dendrites are not insulated.

01:06 , dendrites are sort of like leaky and the maximal current that you may

01:14 here either through synaptic stimulation, depolarization an electrical electro physiological stimulation, the

01:23 immediately at the point of stimulation, will have very large depolarization and some

01:30 of that depolarization is going to die over distance. So this current is

01:35 to decode and we talked about this value. This is LAMBDA which refers

01:42 length constant. So by the time 100% of injection here, this is

01:50 100% current that the side a over distance, that current decays exponentially to

02:00 of its value. And that distance space is LAMBDA is the length

02:10 So the longer the length constant, further along the dendrite that signal can

02:16 transmitted. Uh In reality, dendrites not just one straight cable. It's

02:23 complicated. They have complex branches, have primary secondary tertiary branches and many

02:31 dendrites have voltage gated sodium calcium and channels. So in addition to those

02:38 gated sodium and potassium channels and the of nodes of Ron beer, there

02:43 also some voltage gated channels that will placed in the dendrites. And one

02:47 the strategies of neuron is how to the channels and many channels far away

02:55 the. So, so that these channels would still communicate information from these

03:00 distal aspects of the sounds. So these channels can amplify the depolarization f

03:13 versus just passively allowing for the signal leak out. Because if you open

03:18 gated channels, you can increase more along this pathway rather than the

03:24 just passively leaking out uh been reading channels. And some cells may carry

03:32 signals in the opposite direction from SOMA in the dendrite. So we saw

03:38 the back propagating action potential which came the hack on the lock and went

03:43 the SOMA and went into the Den . And we're also seeing that there's

03:48 of current along the den drive and not always moving toward the SOMA,

03:53 can move away from the SOMA. we're actually just starting to wrap our

03:58 around what these different movements of the along the dendrite actually mean and how

04:04 play into the communication between the So these quite often you'll see the

04:11 inputs that are located distally on the and the inhibitory inputs quite often will

04:18 located in what we call these para areas in areas around the SOMA.

04:24 if you have a strong excited or you have a pretty large epsp that

04:30 can record in the den drive. there is no inhibition, then the

04:36 of this depolarization will subscribe into the will potentially be enough to uh activate

04:44 gated sodium channels in the axon vlock produce action potential. However, as

04:51 mentioned with excitatory inputs and excitatory also a lot of inhibitory synopsis.

04:59 if they're located closer to the they can very much impede with the

05:05 of the signal and the current traveling the SOMA. So if all of

05:10 sudden you have an exci or that's really strong has really largely PSP

05:16 the site of that stimulus and that is traveling down the dendrid, some

05:22 it is being lost due to the we saw the length, constant and

05:27 of the current. And then if have inhibitor synapse here that gets

05:33 it actually will cancel out at the of the SOMA. Instead of seeing

05:39 small depolarization when there's no inhibition, you have an inhibition, all of

05:44 sudden, it's going to cancel out excitation, it's gonna shun some of

05:49 currents and essentially inhibit the cell from an action potential. So that's why

05:58 often a single inhibitory neuron can have really small impact on many excitatory inputs

06:06 this excitatory input will be located more . So greater chance of leak current

06:12 and that inhibitory ones have greater impact this area. That's really important for

06:18 all of the information of producing the potentials inners when we talk about modulation

06:26 uh modulatory diffuse systems. In with the exception of acetyl comin,

06:32 in the CNS acts through nicotinic and inic. So, isotropic and metabotropic

06:38 , most of the uh signaling through protein coupled receptors that we'll be

06:45 It's linked to the amine signaling to by acetylcholine which is nicotinic muscarinic.

06:53 the others norepinephrine dopamine and serotonin, only uh interact through these G protein

07:01 complexes. So they are receptors, beta is a receptor that's linked to

07:07 protein complex. So lag and binding receptor is not a channel. This

07:12 metabotropic signaling that can activate a then little cycles downstream. We can activate

07:19 messenger cyclic A MP cyclic A P produce protein kin AIS protein kinas will

07:30 potassium channel. So they will add po four group on these potassium

07:36 And by phosphorylation nearby potassium channel, can influence the opening of this channel

07:44 it can do so for prolonged manner . So it's modulatory because it doesn't

07:51 open up the actual came again and signaling, but rather, it's modulating

07:58 physiology of the membrane through these enzymes and secondary messengers. So,

08:07 we have learned so far is that have rich diversity of different types of

08:16 and synopsis, excitatory inhibitory chemical We also have electrical synopsis. We

08:24 start explaining a lot of drug effects we're looking at synoptic transmission. So

08:29 discussed uh botulinum toxin and we discussed toxin in the light of Alzheimer's

08:38 which is not really bum toxin. it's cyl cholinesterase inhibitor, but it's

08:44 the cholinergic system. We discuss boum as release of neurotransmitter for acetyl

08:51 And that's for Botox and for we also discussed the G phosphates.

08:57 you can start understanding, we also poynt receptor antagonist such as cr when

09:03 talk about potential. So we can understanding drug drug effects, we can

09:11 start understanding how specific neurotransmitter systems are or correlated or are a part of

09:22 of specific neurological disorders. If we how the cells transmit the information and

09:30 with each other, we can understand those synopsis can strengthen or weaken.

09:36 we really can start understanding the basis what we call synaptic plasticity at the

09:42 of the cells plasticity and the their capacity, their strength or their

09:47 , their sizes. And also this a cellular mechanism for learning and memory

09:55 of new synapsis, strengthening of new and driving away of the synopsis that

10:01 not being utilized. And again, one of these chemicals as we will

10:09 even further, when we talk about means will be responsible for slightly different

10:15 . We already saw that the adenosine we discuss go off in the evening

10:20 that makes you more sleep and they down in the morning and that makes

10:24 more weak. So this is sleepy, behavior versus alert and engaged

10:34 . Ok. So now we're moving to our next lecture material which is

10:42 glutamate and Gaba. And as a , we're talking about all of these

10:48 neurotransmitter systems. Uh And when we about amino acids, and if you

10:54 my drawing, when I drew the , and I said that if you

10:57 at the expression of glutamate and G the brain, there will be billions

11:03 billions of cells that are expressing glutamate gama throughout the CMX. But we

11:10 about amines and we mentioned that amines as acetyl codeine such as dopamine,

11:17 confined to these nuclei. The production acetylcholine is just done in these small

11:27 . And these nuclei contain tends to sometimes hundreds of thousands of units or

11:34 that are responsible for synthesizing all of acetyl colon or all of the

11:40 all of the serotonin in the entire . So that is very different.

11:46 from here, we have axons that project and that will supply dopamine to

11:52 frontal cortex or dopamine to the striatum these subcortical regions. And that's very

11:59 . We also saw that neuropeptides and can be co expressed by cells and

12:04 released by cells. You will find core vassals or secret Granules that are

12:11 with neuropeptides together with the neurotransmitter, and the same synopsis. So they

12:18 be co expressed and core released. , in general, when we're talking

12:24 this whole neurotransmitter system, we already that in order for the cell to

12:31 a cholinergic cell, a glutamatergic cell cell. If it's cholinergic cell,

12:37 has to have a synthesis uh machinery enzymes to produce acetylcholine. So this

12:45 chat cen acetyl transferase, acetylcholine vesicles have their own transporters so that the

12:56 for acetylcholine on this vesicle is going bring in only acetylcholine and on another

13:07 there will be a transporter for And it's going to bring in only

13:13 into these vesicles. So you have load them up into the vesicles through

13:20 . Then these molecules get released in synaptic cleft. And what we saw

13:25 the acetylcholine is that in the synaptic , we had a degradating enzyme.

13:30 we had acetylcholinesterase that was breaking down acetyl code. And once this molecule

13:40 broken down and in some instances, is not broken down. Like we'll

13:44 with dopamine or we'll see with it gets reuptake back. So this

13:49 transporters that will react take this molecule the synaptic cleft. The reason for

13:55 is that once you release neurotransmitter, neurotransmitter binds to the receptors is binding

14:01 reversible. It's not gonna get stuck the receptor forever. It's going to

14:07 , it's going to float off and it's gonna get reuptake back into the

14:11 terminal. While synoptic, you have gated ion channels, there's an ionotropic

14:19 protein coupled receptors is this metabotropic g complexes. But these G protein complexes

14:25 modulate a community of the nearby ion as well as turn on secondary messenger

14:32 by synoptic and those secondary messenger cascades even influence of transcription factors at the

14:39 of the nucleus. So it could really long lasting effects. That's what

14:42 call them modulatory effects. So if isolate uh acetyl codeine from a synapse

14:50 you have that chemical acetyl colon and apply it on the cholinergic for synoptic

14:56 . Here, you should get equivalent just like you would stimulate the presynaptic

15:01 full of acetyl colon. We have diverse makeshift synopsis that use different

15:09 Brain slice is often the model for neural transmission. So brain slice is

15:17 in vitro preparation that is kept You can stimulate the pathways, you

15:23 stimulate individual neurons, you can record miniature potential. So you can understand

15:29 the mini, what's the number of or synopsis have been activated if the

15:35 , if there's a big change in UBB. So you can collect electrophysiological

15:41 , you can collect and measure chemicals are being released during the stimulation.

15:47 this is really how these studies are . It's extremely difficult to try to

15:52 chemicals in vivo in the whole Uh And in fact, even measure

15:58 from single cells in the whole it's a lot, a lot more

16:02 . So most of what we know through from um neurotransmission comes from either

16:08 brain slide studies or more primitive organism like snails, for example. And

16:16 you remember this, the optogenetics from first section? So now we have

16:22 sorts of cool tools where we can the excitability with light. We don't

16:28 have to poke electrode into the cell stimulate that cell with an electrode.

16:32 can actually stimulate the cells with So channel or adoption depolarize the cells

16:38 how adoption can hyperpolarize the cells. know, most of these studies are

16:43 done in in vitro. Although optogenetics really nice uh applications in vivo as

16:52 to localize our neurotransmitters of interest or degrading enzymes or the synthesizing enzymes.

17:00 are two techniques that are very common chemistry and seo hybridization immuno is the

17:07 utilizes antibodies that have a visible marker onto this antibody. So how do

17:16 make the antibody? So in this , for example, it's a

17:19 little rabbit, rabbit gets injected with candidate transmitter from a rat and then

17:27 rabbit will have immune reaction to that invader molecule and will produce antibodies.

17:34 so you can then withdraw specific you can withdraw the the the

17:43 the blood and you can basically isolate antibodies. And now you know that

17:50 you injected, for example, acetylcholine injected some other molecule, there might

17:56 a reaction to that. So you create an antibody, you mark the

18:01 with a visible marker. What do do next? You take a brain

18:07 , learn to love the brain take the brain slice. Uh this

18:12 slice will have thousands, maybe millions 1000 there. But this one just

18:19 to, it's for simplicity purposes to what really happens. So how does

18:25 procedure work? So have my antibody a tag is my tissue. I

18:31 know which cells contain that neurotransmitter molecule interest and which ones do not.

18:37 I'm gonna apply this antibody in the tissue here and I'm gonna apply a

18:42 bit of trin X which is a and that detergent is gonna puncture little

18:49 in the membrane. And then antibody the visible marker is gonna enter into

18:55 cell, enter into this cell and into every cell. Then there's two

19:01 the slice. We will enter into of them. And then we

19:05 you do that. So this procedure a typically a couple of days long

19:10 . Then you put your brain slice the shaker and you apply washes.

19:18 you draw the fluid from around the , you discard that fluid, you

19:22 fresh fluid and you shake it, it, shake it, shake it

19:28 a shaker for six hours and then come back at 10 p.m. you're all

19:35 and you suck out the fluid and apply new fluid and put it on

19:39 shake overnight. And good thing, don't have to stay there while it's

19:43 . So it's just shaking, shaking, shaking during that shaking

19:48 If the antibody has something to bind inside the cell, it will stay

19:54 stick in that cell or stick to membrane protein. However, if there

20:00 nothing that, that antibody binds, just entered through this uh detergent punctured

20:06 , there's nothing there that will wash . So now we understand that only

20:13 cell in the last expresses a neurotransmitter interest. And this is a simple

20:19 that you could have very complicated Again, this is a simplified network

20:25 these cells. And you're gonna discover only these cells will be producing stain

20:35 the cells are not staining or not , not being marked with that

20:42 The really cool thing is that this not really, it can be used

20:47 subcellular localization of different molecules and different and different enzymes. It really depends

20:53 the resolution that the microscope has or have. The other cool thing is

21:00 can do multiple antibodies. You can multiple antibody means the chemistry. So

21:07 can tag one antibody green, you tag another antibody red, you can

21:14 a third antibody in blue. So will have slightly different wavelengths for this

21:20 marker. Now you can apply sometimes even more antibodies and determine which

21:29 in the network are staining. Maybe green ones are gaba cells. The

21:34 ones are glutamate cells and the blue are expressing serotonin something like that.

21:41 , you can build that mosaic of network based on the chemical properties and

21:49 staining. So you localize molecules to cells. The second method is in

21:56 hybridization and pseudo hybridization, localized synthesis protein or peptide to a cell.

22:03 you're detecting Mr and A, you're MRN A. And what you're doing

22:09 you have a sequence that you put and that sequence that you put inside

22:16 radioactively labeled. And you can order for uh for complementary nucleic acids that

22:25 match specific messenger RNAs. And if is this, what I call sophisticated

22:32 counterpart in the cell here, the RN A, the synthetic sequence here

22:40 we produce as complementary sequence will reveal the cells that have this messenger RN

22:48 . So it's not as much of spatial specificity or a subcellular localization.

22:56 wouldn't use much of the pseudo divert in that case. But this is

23:00 second technique that traces messenger RN A than a molecule. So it tells

23:06 something about the expression of, of molecules and specific subtypes of cells,

23:18 qualifying conditions. As I said, a chemical that you have isolated such

23:23 acetylcholine. And uh there's a lot walking around today like there's no

23:31 Now you go to the parking Yeah, that's it. That's what

23:35 gonna do it next time. I'm lock the doors, no,

23:39 no out, no in and out . But um so qualifying conditions.

23:47 we study synoptic mimicry. That means we're going to mimic with this presynaptic

23:55 does with this chemical synapse. So chemical synapse is glutamate synapse and when

24:01 stimulate, it's going to release glutamate it's going to cause depolarization when I

24:07 from the cells. OK. So I'm doing mimicry and I want to

24:13 instead of stimulating the fibers, I to, for example, inject do

24:21 ionophoresis. And I want to inject onto the dendrite here. And if

24:27 supply this piece of dendrite with glutamate the pipette, then I should also

24:34 a nice epsp or depolarization in the . So the electrode will measure the

24:42 potential and there's an issue here. the issue is that so well,

25:03 will work. The issue. The is that this is done and where

25:10 the brain sls the best thing since bread instead of sliced bread,

25:21 So uh now you have this electrode you have glutamate that you're applying from

25:27 electrode right here. The problem is surrounded by fluids. So as you

25:35 these glutamate molecules here, there will a higher concentration around here and there's

25:42 to be almost like a ring of spreading everywhere. So it's it's a

25:49 way to to to mimic but it's very specific spatially, right? You

25:54 quite a bit of dilution, you quite a bit of diffusion of that

25:59 molecule. So you're not really spatially specific with the synapse, not a

26:04 way to really mimic individual synapses. it's kind of a good way to

26:09 stimulate the dendrite with a lot of that has glutamate and you're gonna get

26:14 response but it's not really going to the synapse. If it's going to

26:20 here, it's going to represent synapses and it's going to be here.

26:24 gonna represent synopses here and so on so forth. So, in the

26:30 decade or so, there's a new that has been developed and that technique

26:35 called uncaging neurotransmitters. So, what going on? So here we have

26:44 dendrite with dendritic spines and we know this would be a synapse here,

26:54 ? And I'm interested, I am interested in getting very specific to this

27:01 synopsis because that's, that's what scientists . You know once we know that

27:06 is a response and I want one , once I know there's one synapse

27:10 I want one molecule. Uh I one vesicle, one visualized one vesicle

27:14 visualize one molecule. So going deeper, deeper and understanding it

27:19 at these different scales and micro Now what has been invented are the

27:28 neurotransmitters. In this case, it's glutamine. What does that mean?

27:36 means that glutamate molecule is literally placed the solution here and it's floating around

27:46 . But that glutamate molecule is So it has no effect, it

27:51 bind to any postsynaptic receptors, it activate any synopsis, it's enclosed in

27:57 cage. And the way that this technique works is that you're using typically

28:06 powerful microscopy and you're using UV the lasers these days are extremely

28:17 So the fastest lasers in the world are femtosecond lasers. But just imagine

28:23 you have a nanosecond laser, the and neuron are between nanoseconds and

28:31 But really for integration of signals, milliseconds. Got it. So now

28:37 you can do is now you can the laser to a specific location around

28:45 one synapse and this is your laser and it only on cages, the

28:53 in a very small area. So you can do is because these lasers

28:58 so fast, you can move them one location to the next within nanoseconds

29:04 within a fraction of a millisecond, can boom, boom, boom,

29:08 , boom, boom, boom, , boom, boom, boom,

29:10 uncaged glutamate allowed 10 different dendritic spikes gives you a lot more of the

29:18 specificity and you can do it in dimensions. XYZ because with lasers and

29:29 with the typical light microscopy, if looked on the brain slides with a

29:35 microscope, you would only penetrate through 100 micrometers slice is typically about 400

29:45 in thickness. So you can really with infrared microscopy, we can only

29:51 this top 100 micrometers. But with lasers and with confocal microscopes, you

30:00 go deep into the tissue. So can get the Z axis XYZ three

30:06 . Whereas the fourth one come So you are unleashing these uh uncaging

30:14 unleashing these glutamate molecules over time Now, you can construct four dimensional

30:22 of synaptic stimulation and integration by this . And while you're recording from this

30:28 , now you can understand a lot than just kind of uh confusing with

30:34 that the whole area of. So get a lot more spatial specificity,

30:39 specificity and a lot better way to integration pretty cool. So you can

30:47 glutamate, you can cage gaba, are also uh putting different chemicals in

30:52 cages. Ions can be put in cages also, which is, which

30:57 really cool. So the major amino that we'll be talking about is glutamate

31:04 Gaba and glutamate right here. As can see, it is almost the

31:12 molecule as Java. The only difference glutamate and Java is this carboxy.

31:19 there's going to be a key synthesis inhibitory neurons are going to use glutamate

31:26 synthesize Gaba. So we'll understand that a little greater details uh throughout the

31:33 hour or so. And Gabor oric uh major source of synaptic inhibition in

31:39 CNS. So, Glycine was in spinal cord, there's still uh gaba

31:43 neurons in the spinal cord. But Gaba is dominating in the CNS and

31:48 will serve a function of a cofactor glutamate an MD A receptors.

31:54 in glum, a signaling, there three types of ionotropic r majors that

31:59 responsible for what we call fast synoptic they are very sensitive detectors of both

32:08 and voltage. And once you open these channels by MD A OK.

32:14 channels, they will produce quite large and it will differentiate between similar

32:21 So some of them will be permeable sodium and potassium and calcium, others

32:26 permeable to sodium and potassium. The major subtypes are AMPA and MB A

32:32 Kate AMPA has its own agonist, and M VA and M VA

32:37 Kate glutamate is the endogenous molecule that an agonist to all three of

32:44 These are the chemicals AMPA and MD and Kate, there are specific agonist

32:50 either AMPA MD A or Kate So what is the difference between these

32:57 receptors? First of all, they distinct agonists as we spoke and they

33:03 distinct antagonists. So is blocked by and an ND A is blocked by

33:11 PD or A P five. And will come back in the picture uh

33:16 the score section. So there are gated channels and there are two sometimes

33:24 and Kate is almost considered the same . Although they're different, they can

33:29 distinguished chemically by agonist amper versus Kate kinetically. So their properties of these

33:38 channels are similar. So a lot times you will have an MD A

33:44 and Kate will group together or this will be an MD A versus

33:49 an ND A glutamate receptors. And there is release of glutamate and there's

33:56 synapse, glutamate will bind to alpha MD A receptors and alper receptor is

34:02 Ln it. So as soon as binds to amper receptor, it produces

34:11 in the form of EPSP. And early phase of this EPSP is mediated

34:21 alpha. But an MD A receptor both an MD A receptor is Ln

34:30 . So you need a Ln and also voltage gated. So you need

34:34 change in voltage in order for this channel to open. The other difference

34:40 these that are illustrated is that with the exception of some uh types

34:46 receptors are mostly referable to sodium and . And an MD A serves as

34:53 significant influx of calcium, an influx calcium through an MD A receptor.

34:58 call that posy ally calcium is not an ion, it actually doesn't contribute

35:03 much to a change in the membrane . The influx of that calcium posy

35:10 associated with change of simplistic levels is with changes in posy cellular signaling because

35:19 is also a secondary messenger in the , not just an ion.

35:27 So what why is this channel doesn't right away? And how is it

35:33 by voltage? So this channel doesn't right away. This channel which is

35:39 MD A receptor channel is responsible for late phase of the EPSB. And

35:50 reason for it is that if mate to immuno the channel, it's not

35:55 because it is actually plugged up with . So there's at least one if

36:01 two magnesium ions that are bound inside inner lumen of this channel and are

36:10 blocking the shell. So how do remove this magnesium? The only way

36:16 remove this magnesium is with a change voltage when you have depolarization from minus

36:25 to minus 3030 millivolts or from minus to minus 55 to minus 50.

36:33 alleviates magnesium lo so an MD A is in addition to Len gated,

36:41 also voltage dependent channel. It is to as coincident detector because it coincidentally

36:50 presynaptic glutamate release and postsynaptic depolarization of number. How does this depolarization of

36:58 number? It happened through ample receptions soon as Guam A gets released ample

37:04 open, they cause that initial depolarization minus 55 minus 50. Then N

37:11 A kicks in and now they're working to drive the number and potential to

37:16 threshold values because it is a coincident . That means it's detecting the presynaptic

37:25 release and postsynaptic depolarization because it is coincidence detector. It plays a very

37:31 role in synaptic plasticity. It also a significant role in synaptic plasticity because

37:37 is a source of a significant calcium inside the cells and impairments and an

37:47 A receptor are associated with many neurological and also mental disorders. It's also

37:55 target, a target for a lot uh pharmacological manipulations. Um And

38:05 one of the things that is shown , for example, is, do

38:08 remember voltage clamp, voltage clamp allows to clamp a number of potential you're

38:14 control, you're no longer following the mene potential. You're commanding and you're

38:20 that number and potential. So, this example, what we're seeing is

38:25 looking just at an MD A How can you be sure that you're

38:29 looking at an MD A currents? there is a and an MD A

38:34 , there's two receptors and you're stimulating cell with glutamate. How can you

38:41 you're only measuring an MD A What would you do? So

38:55 but glutamate is there. So al going to be open? Am I

38:59 just activate an MD A? It's good, it's a good thinking change

39:05 voltage. So you do polarizing. you're acting like, but now you're

39:10 seeing al activity you should block, . So agonist antagonist, right?

39:17 you block er receptor with CNQX, no current effect. Now you're releasing

39:25 , you're releasing glutamate and you're in concentration on magnesium 1.2 millimolar physiological concentration

39:35 magnes. You have a voltage plan minus 60 ample is blocked, you

39:42 Liam minus 60 there's no an MD current minus 30 there is some in

39:50 a current. So that's your change voltage that you mentioned that opens an

39:54 a current. But now, you , it's only an MD a current

39:57 your UT is blocked. Now you're the potential of whoops, there's nothing

40:03 zero. So recall that we discussed concept of equilibrium potential and we said

40:13 equilibrium potential also calculated for single So we talked about equilibrium potentials of

40:22 , equilibrium potentials for sodium calcium or and what happens with the equilibrium potential

40:32 these ions. So for example, we looked at the traces or inward

40:37 sodium, if you depolarize it, you depolarize it more, you get

40:44 in towards sodium. But if you the potential at some point, the

40:49 fluxes in a different direction. And this defined as equilibrium potential or sodium

40:57 inward currents. And if you cross equilibrium potential, the current fluxes in

41:01 different direction. Now, for an A and amper receptors, we cannot

41:08 equilibrium potential because equilibrium potentials are calculated just one ion sodium or potassium.

41:17 these channels are permeable to sodium potassium calcium. On top of that,

41:23 , the equilibrium potential for potassium is 80 millivolts or sodium positive 62

41:32 And so what this reflects is sort like and reflects almost the combination of

41:37 potentials for sodium and potassium a little biased toward sodium. And also because

41:44 the calcium functioning through it to the potential, this is what we call

41:49 reversal cap, we call this reversal with equilibrium potentials because the ionic current

41:56 specific to sodium or potassium will reverse potential. But for an MD A

42:03 alpha receptors, we call it reversal because it's several ions that we're essentially

42:10 where this is an MD A receptor reverses. So reversal potential for an

42:17 A is zero or am it's And for acetylcholine receptors or employ

42:32 they are all equal zero millivolt reversal . And if you clamp your membrane

42:42 more positive potentials here, now you're that the currents have reversed and that

42:48 could flow pretty well once the neuron is depolarized. And in this

42:58 we removed magnesium from the extracellular So here it's regular magnesium like physiological

43:07 . So you'd expect that magnesium block the channel, then you would expect

43:12 to see any currents here for an A and the blockade or absence of

43:19 . But here you remove magnesium from solution altogether. And now it shows

43:26 that if you apply glutamate in the of magnesium and MD A receptors will

43:32 . So with this, with this showed that with this traces demonstrated,

43:38 of all the reversal potential for an A, there will also be the

43:42 reversal potential value for alpha and the one for potentials also. And it

43:49 essentially proves that it is magnesium that an MD a receptor and that you

43:55 need both lunate and depolarization or glutamate no magnesium if you want to have

44:04 channel open. But these hyperpolarize right? Any questions about anything so

44:15 ? Ok. So an MD A here and not an MD A

44:25 There's another difference here is that not MD A which is an ample kate

44:31 is an agonist and binding of glutamate enough to effectively open the ample kate

44:37 channels. Glycine. However, in spinal cord, it was a major

44:44 , neurotransmitter in the CNN gly seen a cofactor to an NBA receptor.

44:51 means that if glutamate is only the an MD A receptor will function

44:56 it's glutamate and depolarization, but it function most effectively to its maximal

45:04 If there is glycine as a co that is present and different molecules glutamate

45:15 magnesium, it also has a binding for zinc. Different molecules will bind

45:22 different parts of this channel. Some will compete for the same crevice for

45:29 same three dimensional amino acid sequence to in like a key into the

45:36 And that's exactly what this illustrates. they have their own distinct locks.

45:41 can think of these receptor channels as , but instead of having one handle

45:47 one lock, it has multiple one of them is sort of like

45:53 master lock, it can open the . But the other one glycine is

45:59 if you turn another lock and it fully open the door, swing it

46:04 . And then there's another call for to fit in that door and that

46:10 shuts the door. It's like fits the lock. A lot of uh

46:16 agents will be targeting uh an MD receptor. So we mentioned very briefly

46:22 we talked about acetyl cholinesterase inhibitors and disease. I mentioned that there is

46:30 memantine, a drug called memantine that open an MD A receptors that is

46:36 used as a medication for Alzheimer's but also a lot of illicit illegal

46:43 recreational drugs will be targeting the same . So and then the A receptor

46:50 also a target of PC P. this is why I always stress,

46:56 example, natural molecules or endogenous molecules synthetic molecules of synthetic drugs. From

47:05 we talk about ph ecological perspective, pharmacological companies, they do these things

47:13 studies of safety and efficacy. In , maybe we already have this

47:19 But in a lot of times, don't know exactly the mechanisms of action

47:25 a given drug. So that's not point of the pharmaceutical companies. They

47:31 to know how the drugs act, want to know the basic mechanisms of

47:35 and cellular mechanisms of action. But does your doctor really care about?

47:40 does the Pharma company really care about they have a molecule that's really effective

47:49 controlling disease, X disease, Y Z. So if you have a

47:56 that stops nausea, do you really for the mechanism of action if it's

48:00 and effective and it stops nausea? if there is five mechanisms of

48:06 And then two years later, there's mechanisms of action for that molecule.

48:10 that mean that drug all of a disappears? No, because it is

48:14 safe and ef applications. So it's scientists, the basic scientists and basic

48:22 that really care about the mechanisms of . What happens when this molecule

48:28 Is there cyclic MP goes to this and that goes inside the cell.

48:32 gonna spend five years chasing a molecule the cell. You know, for

48:37 companies that want to deliver safe and drugs and think about this, if

48:42 needed to know every mechanism of action every molecule and every dog in the

48:49 , that would take forever just to a drug that is affected. It

48:54 takes 10 years to deliver a neuro , 10 years and about a billion

49:01 , 95% will fail at stage three trials. So only 5% or so

49:09 the drugs that go from rats. two clinical trials to humans, stage

49:15 clinical trials because we're not quite like and we don't do things same

49:20 Exactly. But these are the systems we rely on and the models that

49:24 rely on to put drugs later into . And so there's a safe and

49:30 drugs, a lot of endogenous a lot of natural substances in

49:35 a lot of things that are pharma , they will mine to substances and

49:40 will leave them. So there will reversible agonists or reversible antagonists. And

49:45 problem with a lot of synthetic stuff there's a lot of synthetic stuff and

49:49 widely available in every gas station. can buy synthetic cannabinoids, delta eight

49:55 10 HHCTHCP. It's all synthetic It means that they have, they

50:02 come from natural substances that are made chemicals in the lab, putting two

50:06 together. And we don't know a about when we synthesize molecules that have

50:14 , have not been researched for safety efficacy studies. A lot of them

50:18 out to be really potent agonists. if a natural molecule as a small

50:26 affinity and binds to that receptor and dissociates a synthetic agent may bind to

50:34 receptor and stick to it for For example, they have these irreversible

50:42 of sticking to the receptor proteins and channels. In this case. And

50:50 consequence of, for example, using CPA single time, it's an intoxicating

50:56 can actually set up a chronic chain events can upset this N MD A

51:03 and MD A receptor function for a time. So single use of these

51:08 can use can lead to acute schizophrenia by development of chronic psychosis symptoms.

51:16 , whatever is synthetic out there. the problem with synthetics is that they're

51:22 to, to make in the There's a lot of different chemicals and

51:27 variations, different carbons and tags, can add on molecules to make them

51:34 potent to cost people, higher really high highs and hallucinate hallucinations.

51:43 then yet there are other substances like causing drugs that have therapeutic and positive

51:52 too. And we'll talk a little about that next lecture. So,

51:57 life cycle, glutamate gets again released neurons. It will target ionotropic

52:06 esophagus, plus synoptic metabotropic glutamate receptor . Once it gets released in the

52:12 will get transported through glutamate transporters back neuron, it will get uploaded into

52:19 with glutamate transporters and it will get in the synapse. Well, what

52:31 happens? Remember the tripartite synopsis is cell right here. This is racy

52:39 have their own glutamate transporters. So will slurp up glutamate through the glutamate

52:48 . They will use glutamine synthetase to glutamine. They will transport this glutamine

52:59 and neurons will transport the glutamine in preoptic terminals. They will turn this

53:07 into glutamate using a TP and glutamine that glutamate will get loaded up into

53:15 vasic call and then release pre So, glia has a very significant

53:25 in regulating the amount of available glutamate neurons and also gaba it's not unique

53:32 glutamate. The same happens with Leo will slurp up Gaba as well

53:38 reprocess it and I'll tell you about in a second. Now, you

53:44 also understand what, what would happen the system if my astrocyte glutamate transporter

53:52 not working. What do you think happen in this and that there will

54:01 more glutamate here because it's not being , it's not being cycled. If

54:08 is more glutamate here, what happens the cells immediately? The sauce will

54:15 too much glutamate. But after some , there might be a decrease in

54:21 because part of the glutamate synthesis is through glutamine cycle, you know.

54:28 , all right. So I forgot copy this slide for you. But

54:38 would like to tell you that the thing is happening with, with,

54:43 gaba cycling. And uh let me , maybe it's a good time to

54:49 it now or maybe I'll come back it and do it. Yeah,

54:51 do it next lecture. So we'll finish up, continue on the

54:56 So in addition to the ND A that are inotropic and MD, a

55:03 source of calcium, you also have groups of metabotropic glutamate receptors. So

55:07 first one is oy and then group and group three are three synoptic.

55:14 it's the same molecule. But remember the fact of that molecule depends on

55:19 type of the receptor that molecule binds where it is located. Guess what

55:26 it's located pre cynically, is it really interfere with integration at the level

55:31 the SOMA or is it going to be affecting more of the exocytosis and

55:37 release? Yes, indeed. It's right here, presynaptic. So its

55:43 , the fact that these um Glu two and then GLU R three groups

55:47 be to control the release of its own release, its own glutamate

55:54 . So it's like almost like a feedback system. It can also uh

56:00 the exocytosis altogether. So that's why a negative feedback system. And what

56:06 it doing poop? Is that going control neurotransmitter muscle release? No,

56:12 likely it's going to be affecting some cellular cascade, cellular messengers and modulating

56:20 overall cellular molecular activity inside those Yeah. Is it just the astrocyte

56:26 regulates another variability or is there other cells? No, it's mostly Asy

56:34 . Yeah, they are part of tripartite synapse and that's where you have

56:39 glutamate transporters, you have them on . So I don't know if anybody

56:43 found them on Myco glia, not my knowledge. So, yeah,

56:50 they're in ostracized and now we're moving to Gaba. So remember that when

56:58 stimulate glutamate synopsis, you get Yeah. So if you have a

57:12 and it's epsp. Hm. If have Gaba, it's IP SB it's

57:28 to be hyper polarization if you have , this is sodium coming in

57:36 And this is potassium coming out through A and MD A receptor channels similar

57:42 we saw an action potential. But we're talking about Epsp and they're not

57:48 gated channels except for an MD A , partly voltage gated. Here you

57:53 influx of chloride for IP SP. when Gaba binds to, in this

58:00 , ionotropic Gaba receptor channel, Gaba cause influx of chloride. Gaba is

58:07 agonist. An influx of chloride will the inhibition. Gama media's most synaptic

58:19 in CNS. Last media's non Gaba inhibition which is mostly in the spinal

58:27 . Other molecules also just like an A receptor. Any one of these

58:33 in Gaba A receptor channel, it's huge target as well for pharmacological

58:41 Benzodiazepines arbitrates neuro steroids, ethanol, all agonists. All of these molecules

58:54 increase the levels of inhibition in the . Remember that you have certain amount

59:00 excitation. Certain amount of inhibitions are bombarded, depolarize, hyperpolarize, depolarized

59:06 and action potential. Go back to memory, hyperpolarize, depolarized, depolarize

59:11 fire and action potential. It goes and on and on this fluctuation.

59:15 it's happening within a certain dynamic right? You don't have your me

59:21 potential fluctuating positive 200 millivolts and sitting for two hours and then coming down

59:26 negative 200 millivolts for two hours. death within minutes of this village but

59:32 all of these substances increase inhibition which is alcohol. This is my

59:40 because you can talk about this and , almost everybody can relate to it

59:45 the sense that if you don't uh , you have observed this behavior before

59:52 when the person has one or two . But you know, pretty civilized

59:57 of wine and the lesser through uh binds to Gaba and it increases in

60:05 condition. So the person is sitting , you know, swing their

60:08 pretty chill reading a book. 10 later. What's happening? Shirt is

60:20 off dancing on the table. No , complete this inhibition. And you

60:28 literally desensitize these uh receptor channels. little bit of alcohol, you increase

60:34 addition to give it a lot of or ethanol is, ah, it's

60:38 , oh, I can't do anything . So there's no inhibition. The

60:45 are really strong sedatives, very uh are anti seizure medications, epilepsy,

60:54 or epilepsy, medications, barbiturates and will have equivalent of the fact that

61:00 have a doctor also. Uh you'll rap songs with Benzo thats Benzo

61:06 So it's referring to Benzodiazepines and it a medication. Uh you know,

61:14 people get into other people's uh medicine sometimes for no good reasons. All

61:22 . So this is the story of , increasing inhibition. But what else

61:27 Gaba do? Gaba also binds to B receptor and it's showing like whoa

61:34 is really significant what happens to Gaba receptor. So, poop the Gaba

61:39 receptor will interact with potassium channel and potassium channel positive charge leaving is gonna

61:50 second potential. This is Gaba A there early depolarization and this is Gaba

62:00 which is a late uh I'm uh hyper polarization and condition. Uh

62:06 that's because with some delay, you activate potassium channel and potassium leaving.

62:13 this is fluoride coming in and this potassium leaving potassium leaving will cause further

62:20 polarization inside the cell. Pre cynically B. Oh Really cool presyn Gaba

62:30 is similar to the metabotropic glutamate It will block its own exocytosis if

62:37 an auto receptor. So it will exocytosis because it will block influx of

62:46 through coptic. It also has this effect on an MD A receptor.

62:54 uh actually activation of an MD A . Gabba A will have its own

63:02 muscle. All Gabba B, Baclofen will be bicuculline and facin for Gabba

63:10 and Gabba B respectively. And this typically what we would see in neuronal

63:21 most of the time. The way experiments are done where in the brain

63:30 , most of the time, the experiments are done is that you place

63:34 the stimulating electrode and that stimulating electrode sizable. OK. And you're stimulating

63:41 number of fibers. So there's going be potentially hundreds of axons underneath here

63:49 and you're recording from your solid interest , this little cell in your

63:55 So when you stimulate these fibers and is what we observed. And this

64:00 part of my graduate work. We looking at the development from the retina

64:06 the thalamus of the visual inputs where would stimulate these fibers, the

64:11 the recording electrode would, this is stimulation right here with a little bit

64:17 synaptic chemical delay, we would see E TSB and then with a little

64:23 more delay, this EPSP is But yeah, but hey, and

64:36 B IP sp. OK. So does that tell you that tells you

64:43 you're trying to stimulate this neuron and neuron produces an EPSB. But immediately

64:51 is also Gaba release in this bundle fibers and you get inhibition. So

64:57 Gaba and Gaba B is keeping the of this Epsp in check sort of

65:04 the reins on the horse. It's as soon as it goes up

65:07 there's also inhibitory activation and goes pull . We're going back to hyperpolarize and

65:15 . A first followed by activation with tropic Gaba B later, right?

65:21 we have this significant hyper polarization and why we say that inhibition sculpts,

65:30 . It is like a sculptor. if you take away inhibition in this

65:35 , you apply by Kulin. This my Kulin which is uh antagonist or

65:43 A. OK. So you block A. This is what happens.

65:52 no sculpting. This is bicuculline traces excited response becomes massive, almost

66:01 And that tells you how precise his range is how you always have to

66:08 excitation inhibition. In order to have signaling. If you start losing

66:14 you start getting what we call hyper in the brain. Hyperexcitability is not

66:21 very good thing because it will open a lot of anor it will cause

66:27 of calcium and can lead to what call glutamate. And calcium like

66:33 Too much glutamate and too much calcium become toxic to the, to,

66:38 , to themselves, to neurons and essentially lead to cell death eventually.

66:47 . So this diagram puts everything together we are out of time. And

66:53 think that maybe if you guys uh a little bit of ethanol this

66:58 you will think about gamma um in uh before you reach the disinhibition

67:08 uh or C to aid the adenosine or, or block the R.

67:14 think about these things, I'm gonna those three lectures. So if you

67:19 to review them over the weekend, can and I'll leave the slide for

67:23 week because I think it is going be a good refresher of everything we've

67:28 today before we move into the final of neural transmission. Thank you very

67:34 all for being here.

-
+