© Distribution of this video is restricted by its owner
Transcript ×
Auto highlight
Font-size
00:04 So this is lecture two of And we talked about this mind body

00:11 . That was a problem because people that mind is somewhere else and the

00:20 is separate from the mind. Renee the cars in the Western

00:24 Although this mind, body distinction uh disputed everywhere. It's being talked about

00:31 around the world. But in the world is Rene de Carte. And

00:35 describes what we talked about reflexive And then he talks about behaviors that

00:42 non reflexive. And his understanding of mind is that it's some sort of

00:48 form of energy that has to enter the eyes in contact with the pineal

00:56 in order to turn on the fluids the gasses in the ventricles. And

01:03 that, because the we have the localization of the brain function, we

01:08 the ventricle is where the brain function localized from there. Those nerves which

01:14 thought as pipes will pump the gasses the fluids to move the muscles and

01:20 execute the brain command, so to . So there is no dispute in

01:25 modern neuroscience and in the modern age this mind is a function of a

01:31 . It does come from the It comes from these neuronal networks that

01:36 intricately interconnected throughout the brain. And is how emotions come about. Because

01:42 we damage certain parts of the you can lose emotional response may not

01:49 the same emotional range. Uh So other parts of the brain that will

01:57 other losses of function, so to . But we understand that if there

02:02 a loss of that matter, there's loss of that neural circuit, there's

02:08 loss of function. So when there no matter, there is no mind

02:14 you can close your eyes and not any external stimuli and you still have

02:19 mind. So it does not necessarily to connect with something outside like rene

02:25 thought. But he has taken this of reflexive behavior and fluid mechanical model

02:33 the body and off the brain quite in his discussions. Now in the

02:40 century, it's Luigi Galvani in the eighties at the University of Bologna that

02:47 these current delivered uh through Leiden Jar current delivered through a rotating static

02:55 It is basically an electrical generator. what he does is he dissects the

03:01 and he dissects the frog. So exposes the nerve onto a muscle of

03:06 frog and he shocks the muscle and muscle contracts. So he uses electricity

03:12 shock the muscle and the muscle contracts then he uses the same electricity to

03:18 the nerve. And when he shocks nerve, the muscle contracts again.

03:23 now he has two, he has two excitable types of tissues. And

03:29 date, there are two excitable tissues the body, the muscle tissue and

03:34 nervous tissue. Ok. So we have that and we now know that

03:41 are not water pipes or channels as . And many of his contemporaries have

03:46 , but they are indeed electrical They can generate electricity and they're conductors

03:53 that electricity, they can conduct that , the nerves can generate and conduct

04:00 . And this is sort of an of wires running through the body and

04:05 are very much like wires carrying the , the electrical current that then gets

04:11 into chemical reactions with synoptic transmission. right, neuroscience, 19th century.

04:19 we're getting into even more detail. looking at major divisions of the central

04:25 system. So we are trying to this brain and brain stem and spinal

04:31 , the structure that looks like one take it apart into pieces. And

04:37 understand that the C MS is comprised the cerebrum, cerebral hemispheres, cerebellum

04:45 the back of the head, brain on which cerebrum sits upon and then

04:51 cord, it starts right here in cervical vertebra and extends down spinal cord

04:58 to lumbar two lumbar three, your vertebra here. And from there,

05:03 radiates out. Uh there's a bundle nerves, it's called cardi Alina or

05:09 stale into the lower extremities. And be and MCY is some of the

05:15 is now looking at the details of anatomy. And when you expose the

05:20 cord, you notice that the spinal is sitting here. This is the

05:25 , it's called the natural size. the front here. You have the

05:29 that's surrounding the spinal cord and protecting spinal cord. And the backside is

05:35 to as the dorsal side. And they dissect the spinal cord, they

05:39 the spinal nerves radiating out in between vertebra. There's a spinal nerve that

05:46 out. Now they start dissecting it and they realize that the spinal

05:51 although it looks like one bundle, contains the apparent sensory component apparent is

05:58 information to the central nervous system. these are the sensory neurons whose SOMA

06:05 located outside the spinal cord in this . Here, those are the SOMA

06:11 what we call dorsal root ganglion cells the sensory cells. Those sensory cells

06:17 information of touch, temperature, chemical stimulation from the neck down.

06:25 everything that is concerning is SOMA of information from the head down is processed

06:33 the spinal cord, everything from head and information from the head and the

06:40 is processed by a number of nerves are called cranial nerves in the brain

06:45 that we will study to a certain in this course, also all of

06:51 information. So now you have information in, it's apparent information and from

06:57 dorsal side, that's where axons come , they contact motor neurons in the

07:02 cord. And you have E bar the ones that leave the spinal or

07:07 central nervous system and form the axons a part of the same nerve bundle

07:14 they control the contraction of the muscles the movement of the skeletal muscles and

07:21 bones and such. So you have somatosensory component, the sensory component on

07:28 dorsal side and on the ventral you have a motor command component.

07:34 interesting studies can happen now because we do human dissections and anatomy. We

07:39 cut the nerves on the dorsal side ventral side and understand what kind of

07:45 they subserve sensory versus motor. You the nerves on the dorsal side,

07:49 won't be any feeling of sensation. you cut the nerves on the ventral

07:54 , you won't be able to control muscles and contract the muscles. Of

07:59 , this is done on the level the animals and also comparing it to

08:03 human anatomy and the dissections in the body. And then uh in the

08:10 of the 18th century and all the into the middle of the 19th century

08:17 field is dominated by phrenology. And came about from the theories of Franz

08:23 Gao and Gao's system claimed that the is the organ of the mind.

08:30 we're no longer disputing the heart versus brain. The mind is composed of

08:36 distinct and innate faculties. So our , our brains, we have distinct

08:42 innate faculties. Uh Some of us at math, others are better

08:49 Then we have runners and then we those that like to type a

08:54 you know, so we all have innate qualities. Of course, as

08:58 go through the nurture, the nature influenced by nurture and nature is your

09:04 is influenced by nurture. And he postulates that if you took this mind

09:10 , if you take the brain, postulates that the brain can be subdivided

09:16 many different areas. So the mind composed of multiple distinct innate faculties because

09:21 distinct. Each faculty must have a seat or area or organ that is

09:27 for it. So for example, is addressing these different areas here and

09:35 is actually painting them on the skull the surface of the skull. And

09:39 saying that this area, for here is responsible for generosity.

09:48 Uh Because the distinct each faculty must a syphilid c or organ, he

09:53 35 different areas on the skull. he says underneath there is organs,

09:58 brain organs that can tell us something the surface of the skull. He

10:03 the size of an organ, other being equal is the measure of its

10:09 . So small muscle, you can £20 dumbbell, big muscle, you

10:15 lift £200 dumbbell, big or big , big muscle, more power,

10:23 muscle, little power. Ok. , it work with muscles,

10:30 But is that true with brains? , because it's not all about the

10:38 , it's about the complexity off, particular, the human brains that have

10:43 many complex connections, interconnections and complex area and very complex networks. That's

10:51 we're at the top of the food . Otherwise, if the size of

10:57 organ is a measure that power, smartest animals in this world should be

11:03 ones that have the largest brains in , that would be elephants, but

11:11 don't have elephants as presidents yet. sitting in un or anything like

11:17 So, so it's not, there much larger brains, much larger organs

11:22 we have still more power in the brain and the larger brains from other

11:29 . The other thing he says is the shape of the brain is determined

11:32 the development of various organs. the shape of the brain is determined

11:38 thos organs. As the skull takes shape from the brain, the surface

11:42 the skull can be read as an index of psychological aptitudes and tendencies.

11:50 is not right here, right? what is not? Right.

11:54 ok, let's imagine the situation where is wrong with uh my mother in

12:01 , she's so generous. She just away everything she gave away all the

12:06 she gave away the wagon, got cows, she gave away the

12:10 she just gives away everything, all food, the neighbors, she's so

12:14 . Is, is that true? is something wrong with her? I'm

12:18 take her to Doctor Gao Orno just a little uh uh session investigation.

12:26 phrenologist would put a tool like this somebody's skull and a notebook right there

12:33 a notebook, lots of notes and would measure the size of the skull

12:39 different orientations and he was measure different and he would take notes in his

12:47 and he would look at this chart on the skull and he would come

12:51 and say, yeah, you know uh this area responsible for generosity.

12:56 really big and I can, there a bump here on the skull.

13:00 sorry, but you can't do anything her mother. She'll just keep giving

13:03 the c and that's what the phrenologist you basically. So what are some

13:11 the cool things that they initiated? did they go wrong? The cool

13:16 that they started is they started thinking we kind of just think the brain

13:21 matter, white matter. We have take this brain and we have to

13:24 it into at least you thought different different functional areas. So that,

13:30 is cool. Where does he go ? He goes wrong because he says

13:35 can read the book by its cover that is not the case. You

13:42 read the book by its cover. some very extreme cases. Yes.

13:48 you're born, your skulls are the skull plays have lots use

13:54 In fact, if you have an in the family around, you can

13:58 a finger right here and you can a finger in the back and it

14:02 pretty scary because it feels soft, feels like you're almost on the surface

14:07 their brain. And that the skull they don't fuse for a good

14:12 two years of your life, this sometimes doesn't always fuse and remains a

14:17 bit between distance between the skull So it's true, you were

14:23 your skull was the size of maybe fist. Now, our skulls are

14:29 bigger as adults. The bone grows the developing structures. The bone is

14:34 during early development. So as the grows, the bone around, it

14:39 also in extreme cases, you may heard of a condition called hydrocephalus,

14:46 individuals that have hydrocephalus will have abnormally heads. And it's usually typically a

14:56 disorder where they have these abnormally shaved that look sort of like what we

15:03 imagine are a little bit like alien like that. No, and that's

15:14 there is accumulation of the fluid in ventricles, it's too much fluid.

15:19 if that fluid doesn't get drained, gonna start pushing on the brain tissue

15:23 it's gonna start pushing on the skull it's gonna start enlarging the skull.

15:28 somewhere in here there, right, the shape of the skull can indicate

15:33 about your brain. In this a severe developmental neurological condition like

15:39 but not so much about innate faculties as generosity. So it's a pretty

15:50 science. And I guess if you into the library in the 1848 1877

15:58 see these chronology, physiology, magnetism. There's a little bit of

16:05 in there too. Uh And it's about how you read the surface of

16:10 skull and how you interpret the abilities innate faculties of that individual. You're

16:17 to correlate the function with the but you're correlating with the structure of

16:22 shape of the skull, which is really the structure of the brain or

16:26 underlying circuits then and nonetheless, it's the field for a long time.

16:32 kind of a thinking in this, this journal as well. Also at

16:37 same time in the 19th century, the middle of the 19th century,

16:42 is a discovery of specific areas of brain that are responsible for specific functions

16:49 that discovery comes about from the studies we call loss of function studies.

16:55 , Doctor Paul Broca has a patient suffers from expressive aphasia, expressive aphasia

17:03 inability or difficulty to convey thoughts through or writing. So you cannot express

17:10 , you cannot express your speech either talking or writing. And after this

17:17 dies, Doctor Paul Broer looks in brain and he literally sees a hole

17:22 , how that hole came about It's a question. It could be

17:25 brain injury, it could be something genetically, it could be a

17:29 for example, that happened a long ago. But there's a hole and

17:34 area right now where that hole was is referred to as brocha area.

17:40 doctor Broer at the time, there's emails, there's no cell phones,

17:44 are letters and carriages. So he letters to other uh scientists and doctors

17:51 Europe mostly and sends those letters And he basically asked them, have

17:56 encountered a patient that has a hole the brain and that maybe had this

18:04 of expressive aphasia. And after he collects several brains, the people

18:09 had expressive aphasia and all of them mortem. At that time, there

18:13 also brain banks already. So you store the brains for further studies from

18:19 that donate their brains. And at time, I don't know if they

18:22 asked all of the people if they to donate them or just took them

18:27 as much of the ethics in medicine there is now but there's many brains

18:32 contain damage to this area. It's brochco area. Broca analysis, pronouns

18:38 we speak with the left hemisphere, means that damage to a specific area

18:44 the left hemisphere will cause expressive aphasia little bit later. There's another area

18:52 is now called vernici area and damage Vernis area, which is located closer

18:58 the parietal temporal lobe intersection. Here receptive aphasia. It involves difficulty or

19:07 and understanding, spoken or written So the patient can read and see

19:13 print but cannot make sense. receptive aphasia is receiving information. Expressive

19:20 is expressing the language. What was area for the receptive aphasia? Uh

19:29 or receptive aphasia. There are also other types of aphasia described here is

19:34 amnesia, aphasia, the least severe of aphasia where you have difficulty in

19:38 the correct names or particular objects, and places nouns or verbs. It's

19:46 so bad. Like uh I think suffered from transient amnesia, aphasia sometimes

19:54 where you don't remember exactly. It's very interesting phenomenon. Sometimes if you

19:59 that a tip of the tongue, at the tip of my tongue,

20:05 , just kind of drag it but it's there, it's there.

20:07 buzzing someplace in the circuit and the in my brain and just need to

20:12 it out, spit it out, know, the tip of the

20:15 Oh Then your room was too And you wanna tell something to your

20:19 and then it's like, oh, remember, I remember and you walk

20:22 , everybody left like, oh, remember. So we all have this

20:27 some people will have it basically in chronic kind of basis, not just

20:32 and then it's still the least severe aphasia. The most severe hormone aas

20:36 global aphasia. There's also severe and damage to the language area.

20:42 of the brain patients lose almost all function. Both comprehension expression cannot read

20:49 write. There's two things that we're about here that are very important.

20:55 of all, it's not one area the brain that's responsible for language.

21:00 multiple areas of the brain that are for language. It's many areas of

21:05 brain. When we study the visual and the visual system, we'll look

21:10 the cortex in the area. V that's where we form the primal sketch

21:14 the outside world. But we'll also that there's maybe 20 plus areas that

21:20 visual information. Language is a very ability. It's in the left

21:25 but you have many different areas broke , Verni area specific interconnections areas in

21:32 that are all responsible for all that can do with language here,

21:37 talk, sing, write and so and so forth. So it's many

21:43 areas that are responsible for. It's just one area. So one function

21:50 subserve by many parts of the So just major function as speech F

21:59 uh is showing us something else that relating to the second important thing on

22:07 previous slide is that there are certain of the brain that are more important

22:12 others for everyday life and survival to . Uh who is pictured in this

22:22 photograph here and he's holding this metal , this metal bar, he was

22:31 for explosions. He was an explosives while in the middle of the 19th

22:37 , the railroads are being laid in England. And so what you have

22:41 do, what he has to do he has to pack the explosives and

22:46 using the bar to pack the explosive a controlled manner and then they blast

22:52 the rock so they can lay the . And an accident happens in a

22:59 that while he's packing the explosives, go off unexpectedly. And what happens

23:10 that metal bar penetrates underneath the bone and exits out through the top of

23:21 head, massive massive trauma. He from massive trauma. You agree?

23:33 what do you think happens to Does he die? No, because

23:39 sitting here holding that, that metal . So he, he doesn't

23:43 Ok. But uh you think he a massive traumatic brain injury? So

23:49 would think like maybe he cannot maybe he cannot talk, but he

23:53 see because it took out all of nerves and from one eye. So

23:57 cannot see it was down to But few months later, he comes

24:02 to ask for his job back. he walks, he talks, he

24:07 and it's a huge area compared to area, which is small and you

24:12 longer express yourself. This is a damage to this frontal lobe area,

24:19 they don't give him his job back he cannot control his aggression and he

24:25 control his executive functions. So he's aggressive and people don't want him

24:31 but he can survive and he lives a number of years. And there

24:35 a uh kind of a varied accounts what happened to him. Some statements

24:42 that he was very aggressive that maybe end up being a murderer. Others

24:48 that now it's all made up just exaggerate. But he becomes a very

24:53 case in neuroscience. Certain parts of brain are important for important functions and

24:59 are also important for important functions. you can still read, write,

25:03 everything, sing walk, but you're this executive function. And so the

25:08 lobe is responsible for executive function is for the control of aggressions and

25:14 And so this is a picture in gauge and this is uh a reconstruction

25:20 his skull and, and and uh the museum and the reconstruction of the

25:26 here. Now, this is another thing is that he does not die

25:33 this trauma. He dies 12 years , in 1960 from status of Optus

25:42 . Epilepticus is a type of generalized . So when we study epilepsy,

25:47 will study status epilepticus. It's a seizure. When you think of status

25:54 , you can think of the classical of what you think is off

25:59 person on the floor, tonic clonic foam coming out of the mouth and

26:07 cannot stop that seizure. They need to stop that seizure. They need

26:13 to stop that seizure. 12 years , the NAZ gauge dies from status

26:20 . And this is another thing that know to this day. And this

26:23 exemplified when there was wars in most recent wars in Iraq, uh

26:30 Afghanistan where 20% of soldiers came back injuries. 20% of those 20% had

26:38 brain injuries. 20% of those 20% traumatic brain injuries, developed epilepsy and

26:48 is a condition where you have repeated to to have on seizure. It

26:53 not enough. It's not called You have to have repeated seizures in

26:57 to have a diagnosis of epilepsy. to this day, what we know

27:02 if you have an impact, if have traumatic brain injury, that second

27:07 , which is epilepsy, it can within days, it can develop within

27:15 , months or sometimes years. It's the latent period. Uh It's a

27:21 discussion right now, especially with contact . You have seen yourself as you're

27:27 up, a lot of discussions you know, kids should not play

27:32 , should not play contact sport. should have them play flag sport,

27:36 until 12 years old. Before a . Uh, 1520 years ago,

27:44 collapsed on the, on the football and they went on the sidelines and

27:48 coach would say you, ok, ok, go in there, get

27:52 fine, get back there. Now have protocols, everybody. I'm sure

27:58 of you saw the player going into cardiac failure last year at the NFL

28:05 being knocked out the very serious There's a uh players this season last

28:12 , this season that got taken out the games because they had concussion.

28:18 you have repeated concussions, you can a condition called chronic traumatic encephalopathy,

28:24 known as CTE. And that depends how many concussions you had over what

28:30 of time. Now we understand that little better. We also understand that

28:34 if the person is ok after two three concussions, two years later,

28:40 may develop epilepsy. Two years they may end up with their brains

28:45 Alzheimer's brains at 30 years of So we're, we're, we're gaining

28:51 in that respect too. But F is probably the most famous patient uh

28:56 with a couple of artists in the of neuroscience. Charles Darwin is very

29:03 here because if you recall what Charles was doing is Charles Darwin was studying

29:09 animals in the Galapagos Islands. And Catalo Galapagos Islands off the coast of

29:15 is very interesting. It's very biodiverse it's also the islands that are located

29:25 close to one another. We're talking 10 miles, 20 miles apart,

29:31 closer, but they have very different on these islands. And so what

29:38 does is that he contributes to the of evolution. And what he does

29:43 the Galapagos is that he studies he studies birds, he studies fish

29:53 he looks at the birds on one and he says, look at the

29:59 of the beak on this bird, the same bird, but on this

30:03 adjacent to it, that has a ecosystem, the shape of that beak

30:07 different. So the external shape of body in turtles, in fish fins

30:16 things like that, they were slightly different, but they were in

30:20 close proximity to each other and they slightly different because of the environment differences

30:26 are so close to each other in islands. And so what he discovers

30:32 that the animals that have certain anatomical features that have adopted the best

30:40 their environments. They have changed their features to adopt, to survive and

30:45 procreate, procreate in that locale and particular island in that particular ecosystem.

30:57 that's not only on the outside. also inside our brains. When we

31:02 the visual system, this doesn't show much. But you understand that we

31:06 very complex visual cortices in monkeys, are our closest relatives. But if

31:13 look at animals like rodents, and you look at their somatosensory cortex,

31:19 find this very interesting structure that we to as barrel cortex. So these

31:26 called the barrels. These are individual because they look like barrel shaped in

31:30 cortex. You have a certain stain will expose them. We have these

31:37 and this animal, for example, , they whisk around, they whisk

31:43 to find the food to sense the . It's an important part. Whisking

31:50 is an important part of mouse's life survival and everything else. So it

31:58 out that these animals have five rows whiskers and have a certain number of

32:04 in each row. And if we in their brains, this barrel cortex

32:10 contain the exact number of rows as have the exact number of whiskers on

32:14 whisker pad and the mouse and exact of barrels. Each one barrel corresponding

32:21 processing information from the single whisker. have a question for you. Do

32:28 think we have a barrel cortex in ? No, why we don't have

32:36 ? We have a lot of facial , but we typically don't use it

32:41 same way as these animals do. other words, we don't hair around

32:47 our beards and stuff for food or or better chair to sit on.

32:56 . So we don't have that. what the theory of evolution and

33:02 what he was observing was the changes external features in the beaks, the

33:06 of the beaks in the fins, shapes of the fins, uh in

33:11 padding, in the turtles and so in their, in their uh

33:17 But you couldn't see on the inside the brain. And the same thing

33:21 basically in order for us to survive for this specs in order to

33:27 they have to whisk around. they will have a specific structure in

33:32 periphery. They will also have a brain map and a certain anatomy and

33:38 that subserve that very important function of around and in monkeys. Again,

33:44 don't whisk around and in monkeys, will have a very sophisticated visual cortex

33:50 more sophisticated than in these lower species rodents. So you have the adaptation

33:56 evolution that's happening externally external anatomy, also internal brain structures. So there's

34:09 little bit of an issue with the . And the issue is that if

34:13 take the brain and you cut you can see as we already

34:17 there's a little bit of gray there's a little bit of white matter

34:20 there. But if you put the under a microscope, and by the

34:25 , we didn't even have really good microscopes until 18 twenties. And really

34:32 the middle of the 19th century, you wanna see small things, what

34:37 you have to do? Yes, should like this. You have to

34:44 in on it somehow. Its iphone you can zoom in, right?

34:48 put glasses. If you don't you can see better, you can

34:53 a uh a Lupe or a magnifying , right? To read something or

34:58 something better. You have to magnify , magnify, more magnify more.

35:04 if you take the brain and even you cut it in slices, it's

35:08 translucent and it looks just like gray and white matter and some translucent uh

35:18 size all the neuron is about 10 in diameter. So we cannot see

35:30 with uh we cannot see micrometers with . You have to basically go maybe

35:36 four X to see micrometers with your and Zoom got a little ones you

35:42 , you could see uh maybe not , maybe you could see 100 micrometers

35:49 . So there is a dispute that because some people when they look at

35:53 brain, it's all together, it's like one kind of a they refer

35:57 it as sensum and they were proponents the reticular theory and they were saying

36:03 if you look at this brain, looks just like one and the same

36:08 . And what do you do with , well, there's many things that

36:13 can do, but you can say it's all one. And so the

36:17 is made in particular theory that the is comprised of one membrane surrounding many

36:24 nuclei with a cytoplasmic continuity. So all one and it's sort of a

36:33 goes to the argument that that means all the brain is responsible for all

36:38 . So if you have a little in the right cortex, you lose

36:41 little bit of all of the functions not a specific function. We know

36:44 not true because we saw that with , with Brochco area, it's not

36:48 functions, specific brain areas responsible for function. So, but there is

36:55 predominant particular theory, it's all one thing, one membrane cytoplasmic continuity and

37:02 don't know thousands millions or billions of in that one big sensation of

37:08 It's called the reticular theory. Big of the reticular theory is Italia,

37:13 Cilio Gogi uh Camello Golgi. That's really cool. And that's something to

37:20 day that you can do in science you cannot do it in the clinic

37:24 in a hospital. Emilio Golgi uh to photography studio, our photographs taken

37:34 the 18 sixties in the 19th Have you thought of that? How

37:44 you get a photograph? Like how you take the photograph? Everybody seemed

37:48 there was a box, somebody standing front of a box it's covered.

37:53 goes off, right. Where do get the photograph online? You

37:59 huh? Print, make print. . So you have a film right

38:06 , you have a film that captured image. You take that to the

38:11 room in the lab with the red , you expose and develop that film

38:18 certain light and contrast. You have now on a piece of paper.

38:24 you transfer it to a piece of , you're imprinting it basically on a

38:28 of paper in the film, you're it and then you're coding it.

38:33 if you take the photograph, you scratch everything off, you have a

38:37 on it. So it's a number chemical procedures to snap, snap

38:45 what a difficult time. Uh There's Glassell School of Arts here in the

38:50 uh in the well near medical center the museum district. And they have

38:55 of these old uh photograph developing which is really cool because nobody does

39:01 anymore. It's sort of a lost of art and science too. So

39:06 Golgi walks in the studio for photography he says, what are you using

39:10 silver nitrates to expose and develop these ? I'll take it to the labs

39:16 I'll put it on the brains and something you can do in science.

39:21 can find something, you can find plant, you can find some urchin

39:26 the bottom of the sea. You squeeze something out of it, you

39:30 measure it and you can say this gonna be PC R. It's all

39:35 PC R. It's crazy things you know, somebody dove for those

39:40 resistant enzymes deep in the bottom of ocean to recover them. People thought

39:45 gonna do what I'm gonna go two down the ocean. We gonna pick

39:48 some bacteria enzymes that are gonna be resistant and salt resistant. Ok?

39:55 me $2 million. Whoa, you're little bit off, you know,

39:59 then they go and find enzymes and heat resistant enzymes are using PC R

40:04 every day in every lab, everywhere the world. Who thought of that

40:09 ? Go down search for stuff, know. So it's the same way

40:13 walked in silver nitrate. I'll put on the brains in the lab.

40:18 course, you have ethics, you regulations, you have things that we

40:21 to abide to. So it's not like whatever we think of we can

40:24 do. But what he does is uh brings a silver nitrate emoji.

40:31 Santiago Ramon Cajal is a student, the most famous neuroscientist and definitely the

40:37 famous in Spain. And as he the silver nitrate stain, it's now

40:43 Goldie stain. That stain gets picked by a very small fraction of

40:48 So only 1 to 3% of all pick up the stain. So that's

40:52 cool. Right. From photography lab to the brain, then you expose

40:56 . You're like, whoa, I stuff I didn't see before. Now

41:00 have the microscopes that can see individual and we have a stain to reveal

41:06 morphology of these individual neurons. So ha sits in fact in front of

41:13 microscope, it's depicted here and he a sample of the brain that's being

41:18 and he's looking through the eye pieces on the right here through a set

41:24 certain mirrors on the right here, a tube that comes out here and

41:30 puts his hand on the tube. now you can see the cell that's

41:34 stained on the slice and he can those cells. So these are not

41:40 , they're cellular or neuronal reconstructions or tracing. Sometimes they're precise, you're

41:49 tracing exactly on top of what you're through this camera lucid, it's called

41:56 lucid. And to this day, have a digitized form of camera lucid

42:00 neuro lucid. So he reconstructs thousands thousands of neurons. He's really

42:09 He puts arrows. He says neurons these dendrites that have cells that have

42:14 , puts arrows, axons, they communicate information to other parts of these

42:20 . He sees that some of the are very complex and they're dendritic

42:24 This is like a huge massive bush some of the others have fairly well

42:30 and really trees and branches that may 1015, 20 branches, but not

42:36 branches like is shown here. He really taking apart things. He uh

42:43 thinks that there are different suburbs of . He thinks that those cells connect

42:46 each other in a certain way. so forward thinking that he thinks that

42:51 connections are plastic. So he thinks there's plasticity in the brain that it's

42:55 just connected and forever connected, that connection can be lost and new connections

43:00 be formed. And he's a proponent neuron doctrine. So Clio Golgi who

43:09 this idea and invented Theda Camello Golgi a proponent of reticular theory and Ramonica

43:19 , his student is a proponent of doctrine. So they argue about this

43:28 together in 1906, they accept the Prize in Physiology or Medicine. It's

43:35 important example. Uh it shows the story illustrates a couple of

43:43 Potentially, it illustrates generational differences. dogma there more rigidity in thinking about

43:51 way. Even if you have a in front of you and the younger

43:56 , your students are saying no, actually different. It's like this and

44:00 using your tools to show, it tells you that it takes time for

44:06 to be accepted. If somebody has discover in their lab and they have

44:11 results and they've confirmed these results, them. What happens with them

44:17 It's tough because you have to publish and to publish it, it has

44:21 go through peer review. And if really novel, if it's really

44:26 if it's something that is coming out left field or right field, whatever

44:32 will be skeptical about it, they put the brakes on and say,

44:35 don't really believe this was on. has seen this before. Therefore,

44:40 not really possible. Right. That's of like if you've seen this

44:44 it's like, yeah, it's just different variation. You never seen

44:47 They like is that really possible? more work, do more work,

44:51 more work. So it can take years to publish some really great result

44:59 the journal and defend it in front your peers. So there is a

45:03 difference, there is a kind of established understanding of the field difference.

45:11 a difference, you know, older is afraid of A I younger generation

45:15 on A I nonstop uh all of differences. The other thing is you

45:21 work with your mentor, your mentor going to think about things in a

45:25 way. His own scientific interpretation, can have a different interpretation. You

45:29 still be winners together, they still the Nobel Prize. But also doesn't

45:35 have to agree if you're seeing something don't have to follow, you have

45:39 establish your own opinion, even if may be in uh in different from

45:45 mentors opinion. Charles Sherrington. Shown . He received Nobel Prize in Physiology

45:51 Madison in 1932 he coins the storm synapse or synapse. He starts describing

45:57 happens in the synapse. Uh At time, we can visualize neurons.

46:02 do not understand. Although uh Luigi saw nerves can produce action potentials.

46:08 do not know if these individual discrete , neurons can produce action potentials on

46:13 own. We don't have the technology record action potentials for another 50 years

46:18 so. And we also cannot visualize synopsis. So that's where part of

46:24 dispute comes from. So, although can visualize the morphology of these cells

46:29 , we do not see that space between neurons. And that's where some

46:34 the dogmatism and rightfully so comes through you're not seeing it until I see

46:39 . I'm not believing it right when see it, I'll believe it.

46:44 for a while, we don't see synopsis until the 20th century. But

46:49 the meantime, there's also development of important stain called missile stain. And

46:54 difference is between missile stain and the stain is that missile stain un like

46:59 stain. Missile stain gets picked up all of neurons of all of Glia

47:05 stain. Only a fraction of there's millions of neurons in this

47:10 but only a fraction of them will up that stain. And that's one

47:14 the advantages because if all neurons picked the stain, there will be overlap

47:20 their processes and sous and you wouldn't be able to, it was luck

47:25 that they found the stain and that stain was taken up only by a

47:28 of cells. Uh What Nissel stain good for? This is really

47:37 Go is really good for morphology. is not good for morphology because Nile

47:43 are name pari bosoms which are located selma around nucleus. So it stains

47:49 exposes nuclei. It's very poor. showing the processes such as dendrites or

47:58 . But what it's really good at because all of the cells pick up

48:02 state. What it's really good at showing where all of the cells are

48:07 , how densely they're packed. Maybe are layers 123456. This is lio

48:13 of the thalamus will be a part your dialogue. When we study the

48:17 system, this is the hippocampus, will start being a part of your

48:22 next lecture. So you can see dark bands. That means the densities

48:27 the cells are very uh the cells very dense in this area. And

48:31 you have lesser densities of the And you can start describing the structure

48:38 on the what we call cytoarchitecture or architectonic methods. And doctor Cabin and

48:46 uses Nel stains slices head brain after after brain after brain. And he

48:56 different functional areas he describes this as different functional areas determined by a specific

49:04 density of packing location, orientation of south. And then another area based

49:10 another density of orientation of the Then he has all of these broad

49:18 . OK. So this is now part of the 20th century. So

49:25 went from the end of the 19th looking at the surface of the skull

49:33 trying to say, oh, we read the functionality by looking at the

49:37 of the skull, which was wrong the way to having broad mass

49:44 And really trying to understand the which means function, function and structure

49:49 both intertwined and inseparable, interdependent. no structure, there's no function.

49:58 the structure changes, the function function can influence the structure. If

50:03 no function structure can go away the neurons, it's it's a plastic

50:07 , valuable process. Now we have of these different areas of the

50:13 Thanks to Doctor Brodman. And to day, if somebody told me area

50:17 , it's V one occipital lobe. for neuroscientists, we still know these

50:22 pretty well based on the numbering that created. And it's gotten refined and

50:28 uh detailed since that time. So can we see, when can we

50:34 synopsis? What do we need to to see? Synopsis? Synopsis are

50:43 the two cells will connect to each ? This is CW and this is

50:49 two and this distance it's referred to cleft. It's the physical distance between

51:00 two neurons is about 20 nanometers in . And the best light microscopes can

51:11 0.1 micrometers. How many nanometers 0.1 mi micrometers. 100 one micrometer

51:26 be 1000 nanometers. So 0.1 micrometer 100 nanometers right? So 100

51:42 What is the pollens? Just kidding nanometers who invented this non metric system

51:50 , uh 100 nanometer but the synapse 20 nanometers. Can you see with

51:58 live microscope? No. So only electron microscope, you can see

52:06 And so those come online in the century, middle of the 20th century

52:11 it's very different from what you're used seeing as a light microscope. You

52:16 carry with your hands sometimes in the and look at things depending on

52:20 And you walk in, there's a a separate rig and room for electron

52:26 and a lot of ventilation and heat out of the machine. So it's

52:31 very different setup but it gives us resolution. So only with electron

52:35 we were able to see the synapsis that the discrete units that have synaptic

52:42 thy though. So Ramon Cajal missed by about 4050 years or so to

52:48 visualize and see the synopsis of the . Now, the reason why these

52:57 are important is because without these we cannot tell what the brain is

53:02 up of. We can tell the matter and the gray matter only with

53:06 stains. You'll see later slide that the gain in the brain is mainly

53:12 the stain like the rain in Spain mainly in the plain. And that's

53:20 we need the stain to reveal the , the morphology of the cells,

53:26 of the networks, the structure of networks using cy tectonic methods. So

53:33 have to stain the tissue, you to apply silver nitrate, you have

53:38 apply blue tour and stain that's blue the tissue. And then you have

53:43 look at what has picked up the . A number of fraction of

53:47 all of neurons can I see morphology just the cells. And there's since

53:53 , there's hundreds of different stains that be used for staining neurons, different

54:00 of neurons, different molecules in but stains are very important. And

54:07 in the middle of the 20th when the technology was developed to do

54:15 recordings, electrophysiological studies from single which really came about in the 19

54:21 , 19 forties with very large And later over the end of the

54:26 century, we were able to visualize neurons. So this set up

54:32 what it shows is that we have brain sliced preparation. It's placed right

54:38 is green light. OK. This where a brain slice is sitting and

54:44 alive. It's being super fused with cerebrospinal fluid, it's given oxygen all

54:51 nutrients. So the slice things, still a part of the brain answered

54:56 brain slide, sitting here, a uh mid brain slice is sitting here

55:02 then we have the light. So you look into this eye pieces,

55:06 wouldn't be able to see individual So we send that light, the

55:11 penetrates through the slice, it goes the objectives and through the set of

55:17 , it gets sent back, there's mirror that reflects that light to the

55:22 and it goes into the infrared camera that infrared camera is connected to the

55:30 . And in very short terms, microscopy or infrared imaging and allows to

55:37 individual neurons without any stain. It us to visualize neurons about 50 to

55:44 micrometers from the surface. So we visualize them very deep but the superficial

55:49 we can visualize very well. And we have these micro electrodes, the

55:56 of the tip of this microelectrode is about one micrometer. And now we

56:02 target individual neurons with these micro And so there's no stain here.

56:10 , there's infrared camera and infrared imaging , without the stain that allows us

56:15 visualize these neurons so that we can more functional studies, electrophysiological studies on

56:22 neurons. This was one of the I added uh here at the University

56:27 Houston uh about four years ago, traveling to University of uh North Texas

56:36 my former phd student is now a at UN T and he, we

56:42 this microscope for specific uh studies. these studies are, are very

56:50 they're difficult physically. So to do kind of patch recordings, as

56:56 do we spend 8 to 10 hours day and about two hours in preparation

57:02 cleaning and about 6 to 8 hours just immersed in the dark room with

57:07 microscope doing recordings. It's very difficult it's, you know, you have

57:11 have stamina. So he's still uh years younger than me and he's on

57:18 right now as a new faculty. uh we're gonna do a collaborative study

57:23 this microscope was just built for specific who later learned uh voltage sensitive dye

57:28 in this course. So I'm very actually because they took it apart for

57:32 while and now we're gonna do something in a completely uh different location.

57:38 So doctor Fang Gu at UN T gonna lead these studies. Now take

57:46 for me and I'll fall back in , in the advisory uh in the

57:52 role. OK. So using these microscopes and in particular, the electron

58:00 , we now can visualize individual And so this is an electron microscope

58:07 that shows here dem I wish it Den Denver, but it's dendrite.

58:13 PSD is postsynaptic density. This is dendritic spine, postsynaptic densities is where

58:21 receptors are going to be located in densities. So, postsynaptic densities,

58:27 it are these round uh uh more red processes and these red processes contain

58:34 found vesicle syndrome. So these are axons and these are the neurotransmitter vesicles

58:40 will contain neurotransmitters inside of them. those neurotransmitter vesicles are going to fuse

58:46 the presynaptic plasma membrane cause the release neurotransmitter, which is going to travel

58:52 that 20 nanometer of synoptic left And that is going to bind the

58:59 receptors and postsynaptic densities of these protrusions are referred to as dendritic spines.

59:06 dendritic spines are found on dendrites of and dendritic spines is where most of

59:13 synapses are formed. They're really They're about one micrometer in size.

59:20 come at least in three different signature , stubby, thin and muscle shaped

59:31 . And it has a spine smooth endoplasmic reticulum, some polyribosome complexes

59:40 the spin that make these dendritic spines biochemical independent units from the rest of

59:45 SOMA. Because by having poly ribosomes smooth uh uh and spina paras is

59:54 able to do some posttranslational notifications right at the level of the spine transcription

60:02 , post translation. So we now that it's very important that during early

60:09 , we form a certain number of density distribution along the dendrite shapes of

60:14 spines, the most malleable elements, spines can grow in size and become

60:22 . They can decrease in size and weaker. When the spines become stronger

60:27 larger. The communication between the two strengthens. When the spines become smaller

60:32 size, there is less receptors in spines, the communication between the cells

60:38 weaker. If there is not enough , those spines will be driven

60:44 It's called pruning of the spines. will be lost. If there is

60:49 communication to certain parts of cells with , new spines will form. So

60:55 really plastic elements here. That's where connections happen. Most of the connections

61:02 . All of that information travels down dendrite into the selma where all of

61:07 gets processed and integrated. Yes. Can you repeat what happens if the

61:12 grow bigger and smaller again, just clear. So if the spines become

61:18 in size, they become stronger, communication between the cells becomes stronger.

61:25 this is an example is if you a large dendritic spine, you can

61:29 123 pos synaptic densities. And there's be a very strong communication between this

61:37 and possy cell. That means that dendritic spine is gonna be responding really

61:43 robustly. And then if it becomes , it may shrink to really small

61:49 optic dens. And it doesn't matter strong the inputs are coming in the

61:53 from it is gonna be weakened. gonna be uh smaller response possy optically

62:03 right. So what do we know modern day neuroscience as we're nearing the

62:08 of this lecture, we know a we can study single molecules, we

62:13 study single dendritic spines, we can single cells, we can look at

62:17 these cells are interconnected to each A lot of this work is done

62:21 the experimental neuroscience level. We can it in vitro, we can do

62:26 in vivo. Yes, we can cells using three photon imaging deep inside

62:31 tissue. Now in vivo, all these things that we do in experimental

62:39 setting is not really applicable in the setting to the same extent of

62:47 It's very different methodology in the clinical . On the clinical level, we

62:53 that we can actually image activity of and neural networks not invasively. So

62:58 can put these coils f MRI coils poor on the mission tomography coils,

63:06 don't have to open the skull, don't have to put a patient

63:10 It's not an easy procedure to go pet scan or F MRI. And

63:14 Children and older adults have very difficult going through the procedure. Claustrophobia is

63:20 issue being still for a long Children don't understand why they have to

63:25 stuff. Uh sometimes patients when they through these scans, they may get

63:30 asleep. But for the most it's not a surgery, it's a

63:35 , it's like a more just more and more sophisticated uh X ray

63:40 Longer. In the case of pet , you will have a radioactively labeled

63:45 injected inside of you. So you radioactive, radioactive literal so that it

63:53 picked up by, by the by coils. But what these techniques show

63:58 are noninvasive clinical imaging techniques and what show us is you can give different

64:04 to individuals as they're sitting in these . And you can ask them to

64:08 at the words, to listen to words, to speak the words or

64:14 of the words. And these are we call brain maps, you can

64:18 them heat maps wherever you see red the active neural circuits. And we

64:23 see that the activity of these neural varies very much even relating to the

64:29 task of language of reading reception and the language and also thinking about the

64:36 . What are we missing on a level? Well, we missed that

64:41 that we have in the lab. , we do not have through these

64:46 techniques, we do not have a of a single neuron, a single

64:52 by far. So we're looking at average of activity from neuronal networks from

64:58 dimensional networks of neurons that correspond to single pixel, single voxel uh of

65:07 image. OK. The future of clinical noninvasive neuroscience is having the ability

65:16 do this test noninvasively. So with MRI, you don't have a radioactively

65:22 injection like you do with pet scan being able to do this so that

65:27 can see the overall activity of the and zoom into individual modes. Is

65:36 possible? Who was saying a I years ago, whose vocabulary was it

65:45 ? It wasn't in mind. Maybe read a couple of articles that was

65:49 it. Something is going on Something is brewing. So, is

65:53 possible? Yes. And you are ones that are going to do

65:58 You're gonna learn all the stuff in and other subject matters. You get

66:03 smart, have great ideas. You're say, what is this? I

66:08 see single neuron. Uh I'm gonna A I in image analysis and I'm

66:14 derive a network of neurons as a of one single pixel appear in a

66:20 . It is possible. I think gonna move ahead quite far with the

66:26 of A I, with the help other technologies and features that we

66:31 We're gonna move quite far ahead in the brain better, better,

66:35 better, better, less invasively and greater resolution and having the ability to

66:41 out and see the gross anatomical changes activity and zoom in and see individual

66:47 being active within those networks. All . So I'm gonna end here.

66:55 when we come back next lecture, briefly going to discuss the role of

67:01 and virtual reality and some of the paths that neuroscience can lead you

67:08 You have a great rest of the . I'll see you here on

-
+