© Distribution of this video is restricted by its owner
00:06 | All right y'all, let's uh let's up this unit. What do you |
|
|
00:12 | you all excited about a test on ? Mm. Just a reminder. |
|
|
00:21 | this will probably be the only time send out a reminder or making a |
|
|
00:25 | . We have an extra credit before test and an extra credit after the |
|
|
00:29 | . The extra credit before the test at 6 p.m. on Monday. It |
|
|
00:35 | at 9 a.m. on Tuesday, If you miss out on it because |
|
|
00:40 | 903 on Tuesday morning. And that's you remember, I'm not reopening it |
|
|
00:46 | so put a reminder on your you know, be like my |
|
|
00:50 | This is the time a SOCA you know. All right. |
|
|
00:55 | so what we're going to do is going to try to continue where |
|
|
00:59 | we, we kind of left What we've been talking about is we |
|
|
01:03 | about how cells that are electrically oriented and ultimately muscles, how they use |
|
|
01:13 | movement of ions to create electrical signals their lengths in order for them to |
|
|
01:19 | from one side to the other. we haven't really gotten to even |
|
|
01:23 | you've kind of figured this out, sure by this point is all |
|
|
01:26 | So that electrical signal gets down to end of the cell. What does |
|
|
01:29 | do? Right. What we're trying talk about here is how do synapses |
|
|
01:34 | ? And that's really kind of the part of this lecture. And then |
|
|
01:38 | we're going to do is we're going just kind of, kind of dabble |
|
|
01:40 | in the nervous system for a moment about, oh, and this is |
|
|
01:43 | neurotransmitters are and yada, yada So we're really kind of taking what |
|
|
01:47 | learned about action potentials and graded And we're going to kind of bring |
|
|
01:51 | together so that we can understand that that's taking place between two neurons or |
|
|
01:57 | a neuron and a muscle. All . But before we get there, |
|
|
02:01 | have to throw this bad boy out because if I interrupted our conversation with |
|
|
02:06 | , it would be like, why you interrupting this? So it's easier |
|
|
02:09 | to put it up front and get out of the way. So a |
|
|
02:12 | is basically a point of communication between cells and one type of synapse is |
|
|
02:18 | to be the electrical synapse. And other is the chemical synapse, which |
|
|
02:21 | where we're spending all our time. electrical synapses are unique in that we |
|
|
02:26 | them only in some very specific For example, in the cardiac |
|
|
02:31 | that interaction there are some neurons that this. But this is, you |
|
|
02:35 | , few and far between. And are some smooth muscles that use this |
|
|
02:38 | of synapses. And here what we're is we're allowing ions to move between |
|
|
02:44 | . So there's a connection between the through these gap junctions that allow for |
|
|
02:49 | flow if you have some sort of to allow it to move from one |
|
|
02:55 | to the next, so that the potential moves from cell to cell to |
|
|
02:59 | directly. So when you think about heart beating and that spread of action |
|
|
03:05 | literally goes from cell to cell to to create the contraction that will ultimately |
|
|
03:10 | their heart to contract. All Now in saying this, there is |
|
|
03:15 | great deal of complexity to it that don't really want to dive into. |
|
|
03:19 | this is the level of complexity. want you to understand we have things |
|
|
03:23 | are called reciprocal synapses here in the is the closest picture that your book |
|
|
03:29 | of this. And you can see , I've got ions moving in this |
|
|
03:32 | . I've got ions moving in that . So basically, you have a |
|
|
03:36 | flow through uh so that we're basically ions in two directions at the same |
|
|
03:42 | . OK. So it would be we would consider reciprocal, right? |
|
|
03:46 | you hear the word reciprocal you give me, I give to you that's |
|
|
03:51 | , the other type of electrical synapse a term you'll hear over and over |
|
|
03:55 | . If you stay in biology, you move further up and go to |
|
|
03:59 | biology and molecular biology, you hear rectifying synapses or rectifying channels might be |
|
|
04:05 | term. And here it's one All right. And so up here |
|
|
04:09 | the top is the best way you see this sort of current. And |
|
|
04:13 | what's happening is is that these are gated channels that are opening and allowing |
|
|
04:19 | the increase of ions on one side flow in one direction downstream to the |
|
|
04:25 | one. And eventually, what would expect to happen if I'm flowing from |
|
|
04:28 | to the other? What's going to ? You'd expect equilibrium, right? |
|
|
04:34 | we prevent that from happening because we'll channels in the surface of the cell |
|
|
04:39 | that the ions flow back out. then, because you have channels open |
|
|
04:43 | the other side that allow them to in and you basically create a current |
|
|
04:47 | actually goes one direction through the cell then back out and then again through |
|
|
04:53 | kind of that pathway. So that's it one direction. So reciprocal both |
|
|
04:59 | , rectifying one direction. That's all going to talk about electrical signaling or |
|
|
05:03 | synapses until we get to the cardiac . Ok. And even then it's |
|
|
05:08 | more of a Oh yeah. Do remember the electrical synapses? Yeah. |
|
|
05:12 | then we just start going what we about what we're most interested in. |
|
|
05:18 | this the chemical synapse? All So when we think about synapses, |
|
|
05:23 | is what we see or what we and simply put what we have is |
|
|
05:29 | have two cells, a neuron and target cell downstream. So I think |
|
|
05:34 | this particular condition, we're probably uh didn't even say it just says postsynaptic |
|
|
05:39 | . All right. So what we is we have our, our |
|
|
05:42 | our sending neuron and we have a cell, we'll just call it receiving |
|
|
05:46 | just because I'll probably say that over over again. All right. So |
|
|
05:50 | cell that is sending is in front the synapse, the cell that's receiving |
|
|
05:53 | behind the synapse. So we have pre synaptic cell and a postsynaptic |
|
|
05:57 | All right. So one is always to be receiving side. One is |
|
|
06:00 | to be all the be the sending . They don't switch sides. It's |
|
|
06:04 | in one direction, always. All . The other thing I want to |
|
|
06:08 | out here is what we're doing is are sending an action potential, which |
|
|
06:14 | represented by the little lightning bolt of flashes because that's how artists do |
|
|
06:19 | And they're saying, hey, an potential has traveled down the AXON and |
|
|
06:23 | it's doing. It's arriving here at Axon terminal and it is causing the |
|
|
06:29 | of a chemical message at the end the terminal right out of that |
|
|
06:35 | So in the synapse, I'm releasing chemical signal. Now, so |
|
|
06:38 | you should not have learned anything new this is what we probably taught you |
|
|
06:41 | biology. One, it might have two. I don't know, probably |
|
|
06:46 | . All right. So this space called the synaptic cliff. This entire |
|
|
06:53 | is referred to as the synapse, relationship. Now, as we |
|
|
06:59 | this is a chemical synapse. So is no electrical activity coming here and |
|
|
07:03 | jumping over to the next cell. right. But you'll often hear that |
|
|
07:09 | or sorry neurons and that muscle cells electrical. And so it's very easy |
|
|
07:14 | say, oh, well, it's electrical synapse. No, it is |
|
|
07:17 | chemical synapse because the action potential is from one side of the cell to |
|
|
07:21 | other side of the cell to cause release of chemical, right. Other |
|
|
07:28 | in here, um like I it is unidirectional. So we're always |
|
|
07:32 | in this direction. And the reason that is because the sending material, |
|
|
07:37 | neurotransmitter is being released from that axon on the other side, that's where |
|
|
07:43 | have the receptors. So the receptors all going to be on the receiving |
|
|
07:48 | . So while this seems very, basic and stuff, understand that neurons |
|
|
07:52 | talking back and forth across the same , it's going in one direction, |
|
|
07:56 | always unidirectional. Now, we talked vesicles and I talked about how they |
|
|
08:03 | up and stuff. And I just to just remind you that what we're |
|
|
08:07 | is we're making neurotransmitter up here in SOMA that neurotransmitter is being packaged, |
|
|
08:13 | down along the length of the Axon it's finding its way to the Axon |
|
|
08:18 | and it's sitting there waiting 40 signal cause it to release. We're using |
|
|
08:24 | snare or those snares and the snaps be able to do this. So |
|
|
08:29 | is just again showing, did I this picture last time or did I |
|
|
08:33 | this my other class or you haven't this picture yet? OK. I |
|
|
08:35 | it to my other class, my level class, they kind of |
|
|
08:38 | some of them left and some of going to law school and then, |
|
|
08:41 | know, don't be scared by pictures this. The idea here simply put |
|
|
08:46 | that when you're dealing with a vesicle has something that you're secreting that associated |
|
|
08:51 | it are these docking proteins called All right. So the vesicle has |
|
|
08:57 | of these docking proteins. The target some docking proteins and they recognize each |
|
|
09:02 | . So it allows those two things come close together and you can see |
|
|
09:06 | what they do is they come into a position so they don't quite |
|
|
09:11 | but they're not quite separate either. they're just kind of in this state |
|
|
09:15 | like we're ready to go. So need a signal to tell them it's |
|
|
09:20 | to merge and release your content. so the the the chemical message that |
|
|
09:25 | that to happen is the release of . When calcium is present in that |
|
|
09:31 | , it causes the merging of the . So your, your vesicle fuses |
|
|
09:36 | then it opens up and then when calcium leaves, then all the docking |
|
|
09:41 | leave and they go find another And so that's how you get rid |
|
|
09:44 | it And so that you can recycle proteins over and over again. Now |
|
|
09:50 | question is how does this all Right? I want to make |
|
|
09:54 | Yeah, I didn't talk about I'm gonna go back. All |
|
|
09:56 | So we're coming back to this picture . All right. And so the |
|
|
10:00 | potential is traveling down the length of axon and it's opening sodium or voltage |
|
|
10:05 | sodium channels, which then result in opening of voltage gated potassium channels which |
|
|
10:10 | causes a cascade of events just like did. Remember we did this and |
|
|
10:16 | one did it and then we did . Oh man. See you guys |
|
|
10:21 | sleep. I got less sleep than of y'all last night. And I'm |
|
|
10:24 | here dancing like a monkey. You , let me have my little cup |
|
|
10:29 | you can play your organ grinder. right. So let's try one more |
|
|
10:33 | . So the Axion pencils travels down length of the axon and it gets |
|
|
10:39 | to the synaptic, the axon And what does it do? |
|
|
10:44 | we run out of voltage gated sodium and potassium channels and without them, |
|
|
10:50 | nothing to come in. Right. no more sodium coming in. So |
|
|
10:53 | can't cause it to go any But at the axon terminal, this |
|
|
10:57 | where we have voltage gated calcium All right. And so when that |
|
|
11:03 | potential arrives, it's not there to up a sodium channel, it's there |
|
|
11:07 | open up a calcium channel. Calcium flooding in. And when calcium floods |
|
|
11:13 | this happens. And so neurotransmitter is into the synaptic cleft. And when |
|
|
11:22 | arrives out in the synaptic cleft, neurotransmitter is now desperately looking for something |
|
|
11:27 | bind. And the thing that's supposed be binding is its receptor on the |
|
|
11:33 | cell. And so what we're doing is we're transferring a signal in a |
|
|
11:38 | way to the postsynaptic cell. So not particularly complex. It's just we |
|
|
11:46 | this magical calcium thing going on. . Bingo. The question was so |
|
|
11:52 | goes to electrical, to chemical, electrical. Yes. So if you |
|
|
11:57 | think of a neuron chain, so , we'll do a simple neuron chain |
|
|
12:01 | we're going to talk about. In next unit. We have a neuron |
|
|
12:05 | originates in your cortex, travels down spinal cord and terminates on another |
|
|
12:11 | which then goes out as a motor down to your big toe, which |
|
|
12:15 | described the other day So these are long cells, right? But if |
|
|
12:18 | want to wiggle your big toe, idea of wiggling, wiggling your big |
|
|
12:21 | begins here. So I say I'm to wiggle my big toe. So |
|
|
12:26 | do, you can't see it, I'm doing it right. So, |
|
|
12:31 | potential down to the axon terminal causes of calcium into the axon terminal, |
|
|
12:36 | know, allows it to come causes the vesicle to merge empty out |
|
|
12:40 | neurotransmitter empties out in the synaptic It binds to channels in the or |
|
|
12:46 | the synaptic cliff which opens up. this case, sodium channel sodium goes |
|
|
12:52 | into the postsynaptic cell which is a . In this particular case, something |
|
|
12:59 | happens because that's in about three minutes then that creates an action potential that |
|
|
13:04 | travels on down to your big which then does the same thing calcium |
|
|
13:08 | into the axon terminal. Synaptic You get the neurotransmitter back to synaptic |
|
|
13:13 | . This would be the neuromuscular junction uh neurotransmitter which is acetic colon. |
|
|
13:19 | this particular case, binds to its which causes channels on the muscle to |
|
|
13:24 | up, which causes an action potential start in a muscle which will lead |
|
|
13:29 | a contraction in the muscle. So muscle goes like this in your big |
|
|
13:33 | wiggle that seem hard. Well, you have a boring picture like |
|
|
13:40 | Yeah, it doesn't be 100% But let's kind of walk through some |
|
|
13:44 | these things, what's going on So part of the problem in trying |
|
|
13:47 | explain this is it's a chicken and egg thing which came first chicken or |
|
|
13:50 | egg. Who thinks chicken, who egg, chicken, egg, |
|
|
14:04 | some triple chicken egg? OK. want my answer to this? This |
|
|
14:11 | doesn't have to be right. You have to agree with me. The |
|
|
14:13 | came first because the chicken laid a egg. The thing that lead to |
|
|
14:19 | , the, the egg that the was in was in a proto |
|
|
14:23 | So it was a proto chicken egg then the chicken arose out of the |
|
|
14:27 | . The proto chicken egg. Does make sense? You're like, I |
|
|
14:32 | believe you. It's OK. You have to, we'll get to that |
|
|
14:35 | another, another lecture. Well, in our lectures. Um Maybe if |
|
|
14:38 | take Comparative Anatomy, we'll, we'll about that. All right. So |
|
|
14:44 | are delivered, vesicles arrive and are out. They're sitting and waiting for |
|
|
14:48 | calcium signal. And this is kind what it looks like. That's the |
|
|
14:53 | junction. So it's not just oh, there's a vesicle or |
|
|
14:57 | Do you see how they're lined up to go? They are just like |
|
|
15:00 | are, we are rocking, just me that signal. That's why I |
|
|
15:05 | this picture. So you kind of the sense this right here would be |
|
|
15:10 | synaptic cleft now, in a it goes by a special name. |
|
|
15:13 | called the motor in plate, but still a synaptic cleft. All |
|
|
15:17 | it just happens to be in a . Now, once that signal is |
|
|
15:21 | , all this neurotransmitter goes everywhere and neurotransmitter will activate these channels as long |
|
|
15:28 | the neurotransmitter is present. All you want neurotransmitter there the whole time |
|
|
15:34 | want your muscle to stay in a state. No, you want it |
|
|
15:38 | be a brief, quick signal to contract and then the muscle contracts. |
|
|
15:42 | you want to extend the contraction, wanna send multiple signals so that you |
|
|
15:45 | multiple contractions so that you get a contraction. So whenever you release a |
|
|
15:51 | , remember it is a brief quick, as quick as I can |
|
|
15:54 | rid of it signal. So you to have a mechanism to get rid |
|
|
15:58 | that signal, we call this So there are different ways. And |
|
|
16:05 | I was in your seats, there like two ways. Now, there's |
|
|
16:07 | than that. So I'm gonna kind approach this in the way that we |
|
|
16:11 | stuff and think how we know everything then it turns out. Nope, |
|
|
16:15 | , we don't know anything at So the first thing that we ever |
|
|
16:18 | kind of discovered was over here in . You do not need to memorize |
|
|
16:21 | different mechanisms. By the way, , these, this list is |
|
|
16:25 | which one does, which not OK. So the first thing we |
|
|
16:30 | is like, oh when we're looking the neuromuscular junction, when Aceto colon |
|
|
16:33 | released into that neuromuscular junction, there an enzyme that destroyed it. It |
|
|
16:39 | called Aceto colon aras. And we uh the way we terminate signals is |
|
|
16:44 | have enzymes in our clefts. So neuron must have an enzyme in its |
|
|
16:50 | to destroy the neurotransmitter that's been And then we started looking at other |
|
|
16:54 | . And what do we find Oh, yep. See, the |
|
|
16:58 | is the weird one. All Another way that we have so we |
|
|
17:03 | have enzymes in our clefts another And this is not the best |
|
|
17:06 | But um you can have a just kind of wander away from the |
|
|
17:10 | cleft. If it's not in the , it can't bind to its |
|
|
17:14 | can it? So that's a good to get rid of stuff. Just |
|
|
17:17 | it wander off and then we'll have enzyme deal with it someplace else in |
|
|
17:20 | body. Ok. So that's another . It's not a good way, |
|
|
17:24 | it's another way. Third way is can have other cells take things |
|
|
17:31 | So here you can see those are glial cells of some form, usually |
|
|
17:36 | because they like to wrap themselves around synaptic cliff. And so when you |
|
|
17:41 | neurotransmitter in there, these glial cells actually uptake that neurotransmitter and remove it |
|
|
17:48 | the cliff. So it's incapable of anything and then it will process it |
|
|
17:52 | by breaking it down and delivering the or just completely destroy it. And |
|
|
17:57 | have to go worry about making it again all over again. And then |
|
|
18:00 | way is you can actually have a itself, take it up. So |
|
|
18:04 | just released it. But you know , I got things over here that |
|
|
18:07 | come over and pick up the stuff just released and I'll recycle it. |
|
|
18:11 | these are different ways to remove neurotransmitter ensure that your signal is quick and |
|
|
18:16 | and over and done with. And can kind of see this in all |
|
|
18:19 | cases, right? You can look here is my Cetalol AA. |
|
|
18:23 | I'm also doing take up here, doing take up here, I'm doing |
|
|
18:27 | up. But so are these glial here's take up, here's take |
|
|
18:31 | here's take up, here's take All right. And we'll probably find |
|
|
18:35 | that this is not the final list that there are other mechanisms involved and |
|
|
18:39 | yada yada. But the key takeaway I get rid of neurotransmitter and there |
|
|
18:45 | a couple of different ways to do . So here we are, we're |
|
|
18:52 | that neuro, we're in that postsynaptic . I should stop here for a |
|
|
18:58 | so far. So good. Is easy to follow so far? |
|
|
19:02 | Right. So I'm, I'm in postsynaptic cell, I've just released acetylcholine |
|
|
19:07 | whatever the other neurotransmitter, that little just floated across synaptic cliff, it |
|
|
19:12 | to its channel. And depending upon that particular uh uh neurotransmitter is, |
|
|
19:19 | can be either excitatory or inhibitory. it's excitatory, it's going to bind |
|
|
19:23 | a channel that will allow for the of sodium or the e flux of |
|
|
19:32 | . So just make sure you understand E flux is. It's a term |
|
|
19:35 | just means moving outward. And if do that, what happens to the |
|
|
19:38 | is that the inside gets more All right. So I see a |
|
|
19:45 | . All right. And what I'm is a small depolarization inside the receiving |
|
|
19:50 | . If I see a small what do I call that? What |
|
|
19:54 | of potential is that action or It's a graded potential. All |
|
|
20:00 | So if I have more action potential results in more neurotransmitter being released, |
|
|
20:07 | more neurotransmitter combine more receptors. And that potential that I get in the |
|
|
20:13 | cell will be greater more. So we have here is we have a |
|
|
20:20 | of greater potential. We give it special name. It's called the excitatory |
|
|
20:27 | potential. And if you look at words that describe specifically excitatory, it's |
|
|
20:33 | depolarization. Where is it happening in post cell? And it's a graded |
|
|
20:38 | to hence the P the and then abbreviate it because saying all four of |
|
|
20:42 | words in a row is really long hard and tiring, especially on a |
|
|
20:46 | afternoon when you're exhausted. And so just say EPSP. All right, |
|
|
20:52 | ESP totally different area. Epsp. the opposite of that would be when |
|
|
21:01 | neurotransmitter causes the opening of a anionic or opens up potassium channels. So |
|
|
21:08 | potassium moves out. I said potassium in or out on this one. |
|
|
21:13 | yeah. So in other words, I'm doing is I'm gonna hyperpolarize the |
|
|
21:18 | . So it's inhibitory. I'm moving away from that threshold that we describe |
|
|
21:24 | it comes to an action potential. when that happens, the cell becomes |
|
|
21:28 | negative on the inside. And so , that uh graded potential that I'm |
|
|
21:35 | isn't going to cause any sort of moving further and further away from |
|
|
21:41 | All right. So this is the sp the the inhibitory postsynaptic potential. |
|
|
21:47 | note you can have one or the depending upon which type of neurotransmitter you're |
|
|
21:52 | or releasing into that uh synaptic So an action potential can be something |
|
|
21:57 | excites the next cell or it can the next cell. But what's happening |
|
|
22:02 | that next cell is a type of potential. Now, if these ep |
|
|
22:08 | are small, I'm gonna use EP for right now, then that means |
|
|
22:13 | a small, you know that small which then kind of ripples away right |
|
|
22:18 | the site of origin, right and it's very small, is it gonna |
|
|
22:22 | very far? No. But if big it will travel much further. |
|
|
22:27 | , if I have a bunch of EP SPS, how do I make |
|
|
22:31 | big signal? Well, how about I have a lot of them? |
|
|
22:36 | . Now I'm gonna use an example I've used for a long time. |
|
|
22:40 | haven't come up with a better one . So you can just know that |
|
|
22:44 | . It's a stupid one and I come up with better stupid ones. |
|
|
22:48 | right. But I want you to for a moment that you get on |
|
|
22:52 | social media uh environment of, of . So back in the day it |
|
|
22:57 | Facebook and you could have polls on . I don't know if you can |
|
|
23:01 | it on Instagram or any of the ones. But let's say you can |
|
|
23:04 | a poll and you're dating somebody, you don't have the guts to make |
|
|
23:09 | choice of whether or not you should or break up with this person. |
|
|
23:13 | you're gonna go contact all your followers 4000 of your best friends and you're |
|
|
23:19 | ask them the question. Should I or should I go? Right. |
|
|
23:25 | then all your followers are of gonna have a really valuable opinion, |
|
|
23:29 | ? And so some of them are tell you, no, no, |
|
|
23:31 | stay with this person. Some of are gonna uh uh you break up |
|
|
23:34 | this person and then what you're gonna is depend upon, you know, |
|
|
23:40 | strong of that signal, in other , how big the vote is, |
|
|
23:46 | ? Isn't that a great way to through life? Right? So if |
|
|
23:53 | take all the Epsp, so what looking at in this picture right here |
|
|
23:56 | what an actual neuron kind of looks you can see here. It's not |
|
|
23:59 | 1 to 1 ratio. Is So the dark purple represents the cell |
|
|
24:05 | of one neuron. And all the purple is that lavender help me out |
|
|
24:10 | is that lavender. Ok. All lavender represent the axon terminals of all |
|
|
24:17 | neurons that are talking to this one . So there's a couple 100 if |
|
|
24:22 | a couple 1000 cells talking to this cell kind of like all your |
|
|
24:27 | you know, all the ones that have a really close relationship with and |
|
|
24:31 | yellow ones represent astrocytes just holding everything place. And so you can imagine |
|
|
24:35 | of these are sending signals that are excitatory. Some of them are sending |
|
|
24:40 | that are inhibitory. And what they're is they're telling that neuron you should |
|
|
24:47 | or no, you shouldn't fire. so processing the information is in |
|
|
24:53 | receiving excitatory signals and inhibitory signals resulting both PSPs and I PSPs. And |
|
|
25:02 | the sum of those S SPS and PSPs are going to create either something |
|
|
25:08 | or something smaller that will ultimately arrive the axon hili and what starts at |
|
|
25:14 | axon hili, the action potential. . So, the sum of all |
|
|
25:24 | EP SPS and the IP SPS collectively called the grand postsynaptic potential. The |
|
|
25:32 | P Yes, ma'am not. So, different neurons are gonna have |
|
|
25:40 | characteristics. We're gonna cover a very few. There are hundreds of |
|
|
25:47 | Some of them behave in excitatory some of them behave in inhibitory. |
|
|
25:50 | can be excitatory inhibitory depending upon which you're looking in and what you're looking |
|
|
25:55 | . So it's less important to learn they are and more of generally how |
|
|
26:02 | work. And then if you spend time studying, say dopamine, then |
|
|
26:05 | you should know exactly what it does now, this process of bringing things |
|
|
26:14 | is called summation. All right. there's different types we have here. |
|
|
26:18 | two basic definitions, temporal summation and summation. And they have specific |
|
|
26:25 | When you hear temporal, what does refer to time and spatial refers to |
|
|
26:31 | , nearness, so on and so . So we're gonna try to demonstrate |
|
|
26:35 | two types of things of what summation in a very simple way. So |
|
|
26:38 | over here on the left, you see that we're looking at a, |
|
|
26:42 | single stimulation. All right. So just gonna kind of point out. |
|
|
26:47 | we're, we're measuring up here to what sort of stimulation this cell is |
|
|
26:52 | right here, we're measuring at the he to see what sort of results |
|
|
26:57 | that stimulation. And then down here the axon, we're asking, is |
|
|
27:01 | an action potential or a series of potentials passing by? So you can |
|
|
27:06 | here, I've got this EPSP that producing, right? This EPSP is |
|
|
27:12 | , you see the depolarization, but it has to travel a certain |
|
|
27:16 | it's going to die over time. so by the time that signal gets |
|
|
27:20 | to the axon hili, it's still , but it's not particularly strong. |
|
|
27:25 | so it doesn't get to the point we open up all those voltage gated |
|
|
27:29 | . And so as a result, don't get an action potential. We |
|
|
27:32 | , we don't see or measure an potential traveling. All right. So |
|
|
27:36 | is a case where the, the of the EPSP is not strong enough |
|
|
27:41 | cause an action potential to be formed that postsynaptic cell. OK. And |
|
|
27:48 | the EPSP and the IP SP is the postsynaptic cell, it's not what |
|
|
27:52 | the sending cell is doing. It's result of receiving a signal. |
|
|
27:57 | And the second one, what we is you can see we have 123 |
|
|
28:03 | uh axons, right? So different on that dendrite. And so here |
|
|
28:07 | saying, OK, this is spatial if I take two of these and |
|
|
28:12 | fire simultaneously. So I'm going to you here note both types of summation |
|
|
28:19 | a time component to them. All . But here what we're saying is |
|
|
28:25 | . So that's the spatial portion are at the same time. So we |
|
|
28:30 | small sp plus another small sp graded are additive. And so what you |
|
|
28:37 | is you get a larger postsynaptic potential can see here it's much higher than |
|
|
28:42 | one. And so when you look at the Axon Hili that much higher |
|
|
28:47 | into reaching a threshold. And when reach the threshold, what do I |
|
|
28:51 | ? I get an action potential? in this particular case, I'm over |
|
|
28:55 | long enough. So I get 12 potentials in the series. And so |
|
|
29:00 | I go and measure down here, I see those action potentials? |
|
|
29:03 | they're still there. So the two SPS together are great enough to cause |
|
|
29:10 | of the next cell. All So that would be spatial two things |
|
|
29:15 | the same time. And just to you that this is a summit, |
|
|
29:19 | we go. There's three, all of them are firing and look, |
|
|
29:22 | even get a bigger one. So get multiple action potentials because we're over |
|
|
29:26 | for a longer period of time, action potentials are then uh monitor or |
|
|
29:31 | monitored but are registering even further down line. So once you get the |
|
|
29:34 | potential member, it's an all or response. Once it starts, it |
|
|
29:38 | until the end. All right. spatial summation is taking multiple signals from |
|
|
29:45 | axons simultaneously to create a large enough so that I can get a response |
|
|
29:52 | that postsynaptic cell. Temporal summation is lot more difficult to visualize. I |
|
|
30:01 | . So let me, let me very quickly a visual for spatial |
|
|
30:07 | All right, if I clap, just call that an sp so it |
|
|
30:13 | a certain magnitude right now. Two us clap at the same time. |
|
|
30:19 | it a little bit louder? How three of us? 123? How |
|
|
30:23 | four of us? How about five us? Do you see how it |
|
|
30:28 | louder and louder each time? Just your head to say yes, of |
|
|
30:32 | . All right, we could do all day. All right. So |
|
|
30:36 | idea here is you can see at same time the more I add, |
|
|
30:39 | bigger it gets that's spatial in temor . This is harder to do that |
|
|
30:43 | visual. It's this one neuron is but the frequency at which it fires |
|
|
30:51 | . All right. So in other , what you can imagine is when |
|
|
30:53 | get a greater potential, remember their . So what happens? I get |
|
|
30:58 | and then we're gonna get repolarization back to rest. But if I can |
|
|
31:02 | the deep the rep polarization with another potential, another EPSP I can build |
|
|
31:10 | my first EPSP. And then if can get another one I can build |
|
|
31:14 | the next one. And as long I'm interrupting the down slope, I |
|
|
31:18 | keep rising up higher and higher and . And that's what this is trying |
|
|
31:22 | show you. It's like, if I were to look at this |
|
|
31:26 | right here, it should be the as this. And so it would |
|
|
31:29 | just kind of go downward right here that. But on its down |
|
|
31:33 | I interrupt with another stimulation. So go up again, same magnitude and |
|
|
31:38 | I start going down, but then interrupted again. And so while at |
|
|
31:42 | front end, I'm not getting my potential by now because I got three |
|
|
31:47 | in a row and I haven't relaxed or I shouldn't say relaxed, I |
|
|
31:51 | re polarized yet. I'm getting the potentials because I am now above |
|
|
31:56 | And so you'll see that action potential those action potentials because of that temporal |
|
|
32:04 | , an auditory or visual thing. can't do this well enough. All |
|
|
32:08 | . But if I do this, that one sound. But if I'm |
|
|
32:17 | get an opportunity for us to interrupt sound. And so the idea is |
|
|
32:21 | building and building and building till I threshold. So now I'm getting action |
|
|
32:26 | that kind of makes sense. All . So temporal summation and spatial summation |
|
|
32:34 | the way I'm going to finish my and they'll come right, temporal and |
|
|
32:38 | summation is the way that we take with varying magnitudes to create a strong |
|
|
32:45 | signal to activate or prevent activation in postsynaptic cell question. So, with |
|
|
33:01 | to that last one, what the is, why does it take |
|
|
33:09 | Sure. Uh that's probably an artist's . But what, what you could |
|
|
33:15 | here is generally speaking, an a or remember has that, that, |
|
|
33:21 | um refractory period. So you shouldn't any at all. It should literally |
|
|
33:26 | I fire and then I come down as long as I'm above that |
|
|
33:30 | then I will get the next one that period of time. So there's |
|
|
33:34 | be that period that's dependent upon that period. In this particular case, |
|
|
33:38 | think the artist just drew them further because reasons. All right, I |
|
|
33:43 | it's an artistic choice and you'll often these in, in physiology textbooks because |
|
|
33:49 | medical artist is not actually knowledgeable about he's drawing, he's just doing what |
|
|
33:53 | person told him to draw. Right. All right. So we're |
|
|
34:05 | use a bad example. But what , but what you're alluding to, |
|
|
34:09 | . So a sustained contraction in a is a function of hundreds of thousands |
|
|
34:15 | action potentials telling the muscle to contract, contract, contract, |
|
|
34:20 | And then so you get a sustained as opposed to like a series of |
|
|
34:26 | and it's really called a twitch. , the twitches are non visible they're |
|
|
34:31 | like that. OK? And so like that, but it's not that |
|
|
34:37 | . So it's a good way to about. It's like if I'm trying |
|
|
34:39 | contract this and let's just say this the activity in that cell, what |
|
|
34:44 | looking at is going, boom, , boom, boom, boom, |
|
|
34:48 | the signals are coming and they're becoming enough so that I can overcome the |
|
|
34:53 | to get the full contraction. All , like I said, it's, |
|
|
34:58 | not the best way to look at because of, of how a muscle |
|
|
35:02 | works. And we'll get to that the next unit. So if I |
|
|
35:06 | have summation, right? If I excitatory plus excitatory, then I can |
|
|
35:12 | have excitatory plus inhibitory. And you from the seventh grade math that when |
|
|
35:18 | started doing negatives, if I take positive and negative, it's the same |
|
|
35:23 | as subtraction, right? It's basically a negative number is the same thing |
|
|
35:31 | subtraction. So we call this All right. So it's the type |
|
|
35:39 | spatial summation. The difference is that signals are going to be. |
|
|
35:44 | one is inhibitory and one is excitatory some sort of strange combination there. |
|
|
35:49 | right. So what this is trying show you is it again, there's |
|
|
35:53 | lot of Presumptions in these photos. you got to remember that not every |
|
|
35:58 | potential has the same magnitude, You can have varying magnitudes, you |
|
|
36:02 | have an EP that is positive millivolts then you can have an IP SP |
|
|
36:07 | minus five millivolts or it might be 10 or it might be minus |
|
|
36:11 | So there's variation with regard to But principally you understand the concept A |
|
|
36:17 | negative B is the difference between those values. So if I have positive |
|
|
36:23 | millivolts and minus five, then my now is no or my GPS P |
|
|
36:29 | not 15, it's, it's right? And again, I'm just |
|
|
36:35 | numbers at you. And if I two things of the same value plus |
|
|
36:38 | and minus 10, then my, GPS P is going to have a |
|
|
36:43 | of zero, right? So that's cancellation is, is basically taking those |
|
|
36:49 | magnitudes and then duking it out and out where I level off. All |
|
|
36:56 | . And this one is showing you here, we're just presuming the same |
|
|
36:59 | . So they knock each other So I get no action potential. |
|
|
37:03 | down here, I'm not going to neurotransmitter because I got no action |
|
|
37:08 | So the downstream cells are not going be activated now where we're going to |
|
|
37:17 | these synapses can vary. All So in A and P, we're |
|
|
37:22 | dendrites in axons, axons terminate on and everybody's happy because that's an easy |
|
|
37:28 | to understand stuff. But as we forward, we understand that that's not |
|
|
37:32 | it's like the most common types of are going to be between an axon |
|
|
37:38 | the dendrites or some portion of the . So, one of the things |
|
|
37:44 | don't spend a lot of time talking are these little tiny spines that show |
|
|
37:48 | on the dendrites. And they're special onto which axons can terminate so that |
|
|
37:54 | create these unique synaptic clefts. So a, it's basically like a close |
|
|
38:00 | . It's like them holding hands. right. So I can have an |
|
|
38:05 | dendritic interaction. So that's axon Adri can uh terminate on a synapse. |
|
|
38:12 | I could be Axo spinous or I terminate where there is no spine and |
|
|
38:18 | is no dendrites. I'm someplace along shaft someplace. So that'd be just |
|
|
38:23 | shaft synapse. So dendrites typically are receiving end, but that's not the |
|
|
38:27 | place. One of the other more common places. I was gonna |
|
|
38:32 | you guys earlier if like, what's over under of falling on the |
|
|
38:35 | Glad I didn't ask my foot, caught. The other type is |
|
|
38:43 | And so here you can see I can terminate on the SOMA. |
|
|
38:48 | , just this is just a thinker . Let's see what you guys think |
|
|
38:52 | I have an Axon up here on dendrite and I have an Axion down |
|
|
38:55 | on the SOMA and they both produce same magnitude of Epp in the receiving |
|
|
38:59 | , which one is more likely to an action potential, which do you |
|
|
39:05 | the one on the dendrite and the on the SOMA when on the |
|
|
39:08 | Why it's closer? And so that ripple effect, you know, |
|
|
39:13 | wave that it's going to create has shorter distance to travel and that, |
|
|
39:17 | would be a good, a good . Right. That's probably, |
|
|
39:21 | doesn't necessarily guarantee it, but it's . And we have some weird |
|
|
39:25 | We can have axons down on axons right, right here. OK, |
|
|
39:31 | can have a or dendrites on It's not a picture of this. |
|
|
39:36 | . We have dendrites on the You'll see those in the nervous, |
|
|
39:39 | mean, in the central nervous system some cases where you're dealing with those |
|
|
39:44 | um uh neurons where there's no real . So they're just descriptive terms to |
|
|
39:50 | about their interactions. So, one the ways that a neuron increases or |
|
|
39:59 | its signal as we saw was through , right? So if I get |
|
|
40:04 | big signal, I can produce more potentials, right? So the strength |
|
|
40:10 | an action or of a signal inside neuron is dependent upon the number of |
|
|
40:14 | potentials it produces, right? And more action potentials I produce, the |
|
|
40:20 | I release in terms of neurotransmitter, ? So I get a bigger |
|
|
40:24 | Does that all make sense that track a simple way? So this is |
|
|
40:28 | we refer to as attenuating responses, responses in cells are the result of |
|
|
40:35 | action potentials, I code magnitude through number of action potentials that I |
|
|
40:42 | The thing is, is I don't attenuate signals, speed things up, |
|
|
40:49 | more signals only at the level of axon. I also do it at |
|
|
40:54 | level of the dendrite. All So diameter matters not only in the |
|
|
41:01 | in terms of how fast a signal go, but it also matters in |
|
|
41:05 | dendrite. And so this is what is trying to show you. Here |
|
|
41:08 | a really, really thin dendrite, a really fat dendrite. All |
|
|
41:14 | So here's the EPSP, we're producing same size EPSP, but because this |
|
|
41:20 | thinner, there's a less channel or for the signal, right? And |
|
|
41:26 | you don't get the signal down to SOMA. It's nice and fat. |
|
|
41:31 | you get a nice fat current and is enough to reach threshold. So |
|
|
41:35 | get an actual potential here. So neurons can regulate how well they respond |
|
|
41:44 | signals by changing the sizes of their . It's kind of cool. All |
|
|
41:50 | . So there is this this way regulation simply by changing how you receive |
|
|
42:00 | . Does that kind of make Yeah. No. Yeah. |
|
|
42:04 | you wanna hear. See Romano. . Oh By the way, I |
|
|
42:09 | make about 1000 movie references over the of the class. Corky Romano is |
|
|
42:12 | rare one. Corky Romano. There's scene where the actor I can't remember |
|
|
42:17 | name. He was in Saturday Night . He, he's working for the |
|
|
42:20 | , he's pretending he's an FBI man he's in the evidence room and he |
|
|
42:24 | a whole bag of cocaine dumped on and then he has to go speak |
|
|
42:27 | a bunch of kids about the use drugs. And so the whole time |
|
|
42:31 | in front of like this. You , you don't, you do, |
|
|
42:33 | don't, you do, You it's like a three minute stupid skit |
|
|
42:36 | him doing you do you don't, understand it? Don't understand, |
|
|
42:48 | Am I gonna have to come in with a squirt gun just trying to |
|
|
42:54 | out how to get you guys excited this stuff? All right, if |
|
|
43:00 | understand this, then we can kind expand on these ideas. All |
|
|
43:04 | So neurons are interacting with each right? They're forming multiple synapses with |
|
|
43:10 | other and they're found within these these of cells. So basically a network |
|
|
43:16 | neurons working together, they can be in nature or they can be divergent |
|
|
43:21 | nature. And what this means is we say focus is that the neurons |
|
|
43:25 | interacting with neurons near neighbors. And any sort of of of, you |
|
|
43:31 | , problem solving or whatever it is they're trying to do whatever the processing |
|
|
43:35 | they're doing is locally distributed. All . So for example, when I |
|
|
43:42 | information about sight coming in and I'm to determine color. That information goes |
|
|
43:47 | to the uh the optical regions of brain, the visual cortex. And |
|
|
43:52 | a specific region of the visual cortex go to. I hate to tell |
|
|
43:56 | this. Now, it's the weirdest ever but just bear with me. |
|
|
43:59 | regions of the brain that are responsible color processing are called blobs. And |
|
|
44:07 | very confusing. Once you start reading blobs, you're just like I give |
|
|
44:11 | and you're just like my A P . It's like law school is where |
|
|
44:14 | going. All right. But you your blobs. And so color processing |
|
|
44:19 | occurs in the blobs and then that is then passed on for other types |
|
|
44:24 | processing. OK? Or they converge other networks so that you can then |
|
|
44:30 | , oh this red apple got like , yada yada. All right. |
|
|
44:36 | it only does color. The other of processing is this divergent processing where |
|
|
44:41 | information is just sent everywhere. you understand that when you see something |
|
|
44:48 | it doesn't, your eyes are not , right? You don't have an |
|
|
44:52 | projected to the back of your brain your brain records it and you're |
|
|
44:55 | OK, it's watching a movie, ? You, you understand that I |
|
|
44:59 | instead what happens is is every aspect the things that you're seeing are sent |
|
|
45:04 | different areas. So for example, the brain, there's not one visual |
|
|
45:10 | uh region of the visual cortex, not two or three or four at |
|
|
45:15 | count, I think it was up like 20. All right. So |
|
|
45:20 | processing which we normally attribute to the lobe of the brain is really the |
|
|
45:29 | lobe and the temporal lobe and the lobe. Oh yeah. And the |
|
|
45:33 | lobe and information is broken down by and color and movement and shape and |
|
|
45:40 | sorts of other aspects and other information then distributed out to other processing centers |
|
|
45:46 | association areas in the brain. So you can understand that the thing you're |
|
|
45:50 | at is a hopping frog, So this would be an example of |
|
|
45:56 | divergent network information is sent everywhere. experiences of anything you have a meal |
|
|
46:07 | before you, right? You smell and that sense of smell goes |
|
|
46:13 | I'm gonna send it to the memory . Do I recognize the smell? |
|
|
46:19 | ? I'm gonna smell that and it , OK, this smell has different |
|
|
46:24 | in it. It's pleasant. So see it's being sent to different parts |
|
|
46:27 | the brain to get a response, ? So processing information can be done |
|
|
46:37 | a very localized area to do something or it's broadly sent out so that |
|
|
46:43 | have a greater, a greater breadth understanding of what it is that you're |
|
|
46:50 | . The other thing I'd add is the more neurons you have in a |
|
|
46:55 | , the more synaptic delay you have more it takes to process because each |
|
|
47:00 | takes time to process. Now. not very much. It's like we're |
|
|
47:04 | about 0.1 milliseconds, right? But you have 0.1 milliseconds to, to |
|
|
47:11 | an Epsp and you have 10 in chain, then that's 10 times 0.1 |
|
|
47:19 | . That's a millisecond, right? you ever been walking across the street |
|
|
47:25 | out your phone like you always do all of a sudden you hear this |
|
|
47:30 | horn. So the first thing you to do is you have to unplug |
|
|
47:34 | brain for a second and you kind stop and you look and you see |
|
|
47:38 | bus barreling down on you have that happened? Something similar, right? |
|
|
47:43 | what do you do? Do you go oh I know how to respond |
|
|
47:46 | this or do you sit there and ? Huh? That's a bus, |
|
|
47:56 | ? My situation appears to be dire you always talk like somebody from the |
|
|
48:01 | century, right? Part of that your brain like I don't know what |
|
|
48:08 | do because I've got to process all info input. And so that delay |
|
|
48:14 | kind of there now. Is it all that? Is it really that |
|
|
48:18 | but it kind of gives you that , you mean if I have to |
|
|
48:23 | more and more processes? If I more neurons in this chain, it's |
|
|
48:26 | cause a problem. All right. I throw a baseball at your |
|
|
48:29 | What are you gonna do? I like that. You move your |
|
|
48:33 | . You don't even have to think some of you, you know, |
|
|
48:35 | too cool for school. You just . OK. A reflex has very |
|
|
48:42 | neurons in the pathway because it says will always do blank, right. |
|
|
48:49 | gonna talk about reflex again in the section, but you can have fun |
|
|
48:51 | it. You can go do your cross your leg thing and, and |
|
|
48:54 | the uh knee jerk reflex. See you can keep it from happening. |
|
|
48:58 | you hit that ligament. Just You guys know how to do the |
|
|
49:01 | jerk reflex, right? Doctor has it to you for years, basically |
|
|
49:05 | that wonderful little ligament right underneath the and you can just sit, you |
|
|
49:08 | just do it with like a little chop, just get a friend. |
|
|
49:11 | them start karate chopping your knee and just gonna sit there and just do |
|
|
49:15 | . All right. Why reflex always a reflex? All right. And |
|
|
49:19 | also a very, very short neuron . I promise you neurotransmitters. Hundreds |
|
|
49:28 | them. Should we memorize them Come on. Come on, let's |
|
|
49:31 | it. Let's not fun. All , neurotransmitter is simply the chemical signal |
|
|
49:40 | by a neuron. It's gonna be in nature, but they can act |
|
|
49:46 | an autocrine fashion. I mean, can talk back to myself. |
|
|
49:50 | you can all right, they're gonna at the synapse. So this is |
|
|
49:56 | message that we're releasing from that And as I mentioned, there are |
|
|
50:01 | of them. This is like the list of the families that we're familiar |
|
|
50:06 | , right? So Aceto Cole was first one discovered, we were very |
|
|
50:10 | in discovering acetycholine back in the early hundreds. And we're like, oh |
|
|
50:14 | , we got neurotransmitter, we figured out, we know how neurons talk |
|
|
50:18 | muscles. So I guarantee all we to do is look for things that |
|
|
50:21 | like Aceto Cole and we are going understand how the brain works and nothing |
|
|
50:26 | like Aceta Coli. All right, is nothing else like aceta coli. |
|
|
50:31 | right, turns out that a lot our neurotransmitters are just modifications of amino |
|
|
50:37 | . So we got the AOM means have the amino acids themselves. |
|
|
50:40 | by the way that A P that been spending your entire life learning is |
|
|
50:43 | molecule of energy can act as a . Great. What about gasses? |
|
|
50:50 | , we got gasses too, We're not talking about the stuff you |
|
|
50:54 | after a very, very, very meal, your body is filled with |
|
|
50:58 | acid filled with carbon monoxide filled with sulfide. Do any of these gasses |
|
|
51:04 | like gasses you want circulating through your . Hydrogen sulfide is what makes rotten |
|
|
51:08 | smell rotten, right? But it's neurotransmitter. It's called a uh it's |
|
|
51:17 | just forgot the name. It's a emitter. So they took neurotransmitter and |
|
|
51:23 | and jammed it together. Got These are more familiar and then the |
|
|
51:29 | themselves as IOS can serve as Now, with that in mind, |
|
|
51:35 | , I don't want you to sit and, like, write down |
|
|
51:37 | oh, these things, I just you to kind of get a |
|
|
51:39 | Are there a lot of different types classes of neurotransmitters? Yes. What |
|
|
51:45 | they have in common? They're released neurons and they cause signals. All |
|
|
51:51 | . What we're interested in are these right here. These three classes first |
|
|
51:57 | coin, as I mentioned, first discovered, um doesn't look like anything |
|
|
52:02 | . It is both excitatory or inhibitory upon which system you're looking at and |
|
|
52:08 | the situation or circumstances are. If want to see what Aceta Cole |
|
|
52:12 | it is up there in the blue Bono. The very first one, |
|
|
52:16 | can see a co A plus Cole together. That's where you get the |
|
|
52:20 | right now. Aceto Cole. This uh we're going to spend time talking |
|
|
52:27 | it over and over again. But just going to give you a really |
|
|
52:29 | one. Aceto Cole is found in neuromuscular junction. All right. So |
|
|
52:33 | neurons tell muscles what to do your contracts, it always, always, |
|
|
52:39 | contracts. So in the case of neuromuscular junction, Aceto colon is |
|
|
52:44 | always, always, always no exception the rule under no circumstances. Is |
|
|
52:47 | different? It's excitatory, but in systems, it can be excitatory or |
|
|
52:55 | . Another group, the amino we know what the amino acids |
|
|
52:58 | We've all had to learn something or about them and what we have here |
|
|
53:03 | we take these and either we have original amino acid or we do a |
|
|
53:08 | modification to them. So, the excitatory ones that you should know are |
|
|
53:12 | acids, you know, glutamate, heard of glutamate, right? And |
|
|
53:15 | heard of Sparta, haven't you? . Ok. So those are two |
|
|
53:20 | neurotransmitters, glutamate. You do a modification to it. You get |
|
|
53:26 | So Gaba, which is a modification glutamate is an inhibitory neurotransmitter. All |
|
|
53:31 | , or usually an inhibitory neurotransmitter. glycine is another amino acid which is |
|
|
53:38 | serving as an inhibitory neurotransmitter. I this was the easy one of these |
|
|
53:43 | is not like the others. I , we've got three GS in an |
|
|
53:46 | but you can see we've got two and A G and an A. |
|
|
53:48 | there's no easy pattern to remember other glutamate and asperate, they both end |
|
|
53:55 | AIDS. So that's the excitatory So you just have to kind of |
|
|
54:00 | them and internalize them. All the biogenic means these are amino or |
|
|
54:05 | are neurotransmitter you've already learned about or about, you just may not know |
|
|
54:09 | by name. All right. So have the CCO means what we've done |
|
|
54:14 | is basically take off the carboxyl So where are we looking? So |
|
|
54:18 | here we look at tyrosine, don't look at tyrosine, look at |
|
|
54:22 | . Um You can see we do modifications. So if you start up |
|
|
54:25 | at Thyro, we took off that group, we get down to |
|
|
54:29 | There's no carboxyl group, we do modification. Put a hydroxyl group. |
|
|
54:33 | is why you take organic chemistry, ? So you go oh OK. |
|
|
54:36 | know that word hydroxyl. OK. But you can see that's, it's |
|
|
54:40 | , you're just taking tyrosine and you're making modifications all the way, all |
|
|
54:44 | way down. All right. But are the catacholamines. You've heard of |
|
|
54:48 | , right? That's a, a word. Yeah. Have you heard |
|
|
54:52 | histamine? Right. When you think histamine, what do you think of |
|
|
54:57 | ? So you think of, don't the those that you get? And |
|
|
55:01 | when you got a of those, do you do? You take |
|
|
55:06 | OK. And then you can breathe . It's a vasodilator. That's why |
|
|
55:11 | , we, we do that. right. Sorry. Baso constrictor. |
|
|
55:17 | , I've confused myself. So I be careful. All right. Then |
|
|
55:21 | have the weird ones. The CTA means and so dopamine is Cine. |
|
|
55:24 | Serotonin, I should have mentioned you've of Serotonin, right? So that's |
|
|
55:28 | one. These are all happy fun . Uh with regard to the cats |
|
|
55:35 | . But then we have Ep and Epinephrine. Um, you may not |
|
|
55:38 | heard of Epinephrine, but you have of Adrenaline, haven't you? |
|
|
55:43 | you've heard of Adrenaline. Adrenaline and are the same thing. It's just |
|
|
55:47 | fancy word for one. And the behind it is that one. they |
|
|
55:52 | it and are able to manufacture but you can't give the manufactured name |
|
|
55:56 | the chemical or something like that. I think that's was why they changed |
|
|
56:00 | . And then Norine is epinephrine's close . So it's no adrenaline. All |
|
|
56:05 | . That's where those names come All right. And again, they |
|
|
56:09 | both excitatory inhibitory activities together. That be uh either or yes, when |
|
|
56:16 | comes to a neurotransmitter. And this why we take the Neuroscience class because |
|
|
56:22 | far more broad and, and goes a greater depth. A neurotransmitter can |
|
|
56:28 | what is referred to as a divergent or a convergent effect. All |
|
|
56:32 | So a divergent effect. And you see up here on the top it |
|
|
56:35 | , look here, I've got this and this neurotransmitter can bin to a |
|
|
56:40 | of different types of receptors. All , these are Agenor receptors that we're |
|
|
56:46 | in this list. And so what I do in this if I bind |
|
|
56:49 | the alpha one? Well, I cause a current are different types of |
|
|
56:53 | . I can open and close different of channels. I want to point |
|
|
56:57 | out here. So alpha one and two here, I'm closing a potassium |
|
|
57:01 | or reducing the flow of potassium through channel over here. What am I |
|
|
57:07 | ? I'm increasing it. So you see these are opposite effects. What |
|
|
57:13 | the opposite effect? Just the presence a particular channel, right? So |
|
|
57:18 | upon which receptor I have, I get different responses. This would be |
|
|
57:23 | example of divergent effects. A convergent says, hey, look, um |
|
|
57:29 | have all these different types of They combine their own specific types of |
|
|
57:34 | , but they all result in the response in that particular cell. |
|
|
57:39 | So here you can say, oh , I've got this cell and I |
|
|
57:42 | activate it through this or I can it through that or I can activate |
|
|
57:45 | through this or I can activate it that. So there's different ways to |
|
|
57:49 | the cell to get the same That would be a convergent, a |
|
|
58:00 | thing. But an important thing that can do because of those axonal ax |
|
|
58:07 | Axo axonal connections is that you can and facilitate specific interactions between two neurons |
|
|
58:18 | not affect the interactions with the other that the the presynaptic neuron is interacting |
|
|
58:25 | . See if we can understand I see the, I can see |
|
|
58:28 | fur brow in the background and said didn't say that. Well, I |
|
|
58:33 | this cell right here. That would the presynaptic cell. In our little |
|
|
58:37 | , the little white cells down those are postsynaptic cells. OK. |
|
|
58:42 | we're focusing here on that synaptic You can see I've stimulated this |
|
|
58:48 | So in reference to this synapse, is pre or post, pre. |
|
|
58:53 | this would be post. So all is is a frame of reference, |
|
|
58:56 | ? So here I've excited the cell produce an action potential. So this |
|
|
59:02 | synaptic cell is sitting that action potential its pathway or down its axon where |
|
|
59:07 | have two extra collaterals. So in case, I'm not activating one |
|
|
59:13 | I'm activating three different cells so So good you're with me, |
|
|
59:18 | If I have an axonal presynaptic um cell and that cell happens to be |
|
|
59:29 | , what it will do is if cell is stimulated, it will release |
|
|
59:32 | inhibitory neurotransmitter to block or inhibit the that, that uh signal here, |
|
|
59:43 | ? So in other words, what doing is saying, I don't want |
|
|
59:46 | to stimulate the cell downstream from I don't care about these other |
|
|
59:52 | I just care about this one. so this would be presynaptic inhibition. |
|
|
59:57 | cell has been stimulated, right? presynaptic cell has been stimulated, but |
|
|
60:04 | blocking the pre synaptic cleft or presynaptic to prevent a response. So I |
|
|
60:12 | get responses in the other ones, not in the one where I'm doing |
|
|
60:16 | inhibition. Let's flip it around. have no signal in that synaptic |
|
|
60:23 | So no action potential. So this is not being stimulated. That cell |
|
|
60:27 | not being stimulated. That cell is being stimulated. But instead of this |
|
|
60:30 | an inhibitory neuron, that's an excitatory and it's activated. So, am |
|
|
60:36 | going to see a response to this or no? What do you |
|
|
60:41 | You're nodding? Yes. Anyone want disagree with her. No one's brave |
|
|
60:46 | to disagree with you. But you're . So that would be facilitation. |
|
|
60:52 | would be presynaptic facilitation. OK. the relationship between neurons is not simply |
|
|
61:01 | to dendrite, axon to dendrite over over again, we can regulate neuronal |
|
|
61:08 | at different locations and affect specific So here, presynaptic inhibition is occurring |
|
|
61:17 | this cell and that one, but in the other two. Yeah. |
|
|
61:28 | , it's a separate one altogether. way you can think about it is |
|
|
61:32 | just going to use these, these here, these two are talking to |
|
|
61:35 | other. She's sending signals this right? But I'm sitting in between |
|
|
61:40 | and I'm saying no, the signal not allowed to go forward. So |
|
|
61:43 | would be the pre synaptic inhibition. the third cell interfering between the two |
|
|
61:49 | that are normally talking or they're not to each other at all. And |
|
|
61:53 | saying, go ahead and talk. would be the facilitation. Mhm |
|
|
62:09 | Yeah. So the question she's asking will facilitation always cause a response? |
|
|
62:14 | right. So let's ask that in biological terms. What do we |
|
|
62:17 | Will we always get a response? , it's, it's the always, |
|
|
62:21 | that qualifier right? There always is a good qualifier. We're likely to |
|
|
62:26 | a response. Right? So, you can think when I hear the |
|
|
62:29 | facilitation, I'm stimulating to get a . Whether I get one or not |
|
|
62:34 | dependent upon the strength of the Yada yada yada. So far we're |
|
|
62:41 | , is any of this stuff sound or hard or difficult, right? |
|
|
62:47 | isn't like trying to memorize all your . Here's an ugly, ugly word |
|
|
62:59 | . What do neuro modulators do? do you think they do? Just |
|
|
63:03 | the name? You can cheat and up there. But if you hear |
|
|
63:06 | neuromodulator, what do you think it modifies or modulates neurons? Yep. |
|
|
63:12 | then you're good to go. Neurotransmitters chemicals released from a cell. They're |
|
|
63:18 | from the cell neuro modulators modulate the . All right. So here we've |
|
|
63:23 | to deal with relationship. All So when I'm dealing with a pre |
|
|
63:27 | cell and a postsynaptic cell, there a ratio between the cells that are |
|
|
63:34 | amount of neurotransmitter being released and the of receptors available. All right. |
|
|
63:42 | let's just say for a moment, make our lives easy. We have |
|
|
63:44 | neurotransmitters and we have 10 receptors So happy. Every, every neurotransmitter has |
|
|
63:54 | receptor to bind tube. All What a neuromodulator does is it changes |
|
|
63:59 | relationship? And so there are different that we can change this relationship. |
|
|
64:03 | can cause more neurotransmitter to be In other words, if one vesicle |
|
|
64:08 | 10 neurotransmitters, maybe instead of one being reopened, I now open |
|
|
64:15 | So I've doubled the amount of neurotransmitter that clip and in doing so, |
|
|
64:19 | am facilitating or up regulating the activity those two cells, aren't I? |
|
|
64:28 | . What's another way I could do ? What's another way I can up |
|
|
64:32 | the activity between the two more? heard the word receptors. So if |
|
|
64:39 | increase the number of receptors, so I still have 10 neurotransmitters and I |
|
|
64:43 | 20 receptors, the probability of a finding a receptor quickly has increased two |
|
|
64:48 | , hasn't it? So, in of these cases, what I've done |
|
|
64:51 | I've facilitated the interaction. And so what a neuromodulator does. It will |
|
|
64:57 | increase, facilitate the interaction between those cells. Now, how can I |
|
|
65:02 | the interaction between two cells? I reduce, oh I heard, reduce |
|
|
65:08 | amount of neurotransmitter. I'm I'm right? Or I can reduce the |
|
|
65:13 | of receptor that's available. So that be um the type of inhibition that |
|
|
65:18 | might see. All right. So of the ways that your nervous system |
|
|
65:24 | its activity is, but through neuromodulation will either increase or decrease activity so |
|
|
65:31 | you increase and decrease relational activity between two cells. And since you have |
|
|
65:36 | of millions of these cells doing you can imagine facilitation and inhibition is |
|
|
65:41 | of the ways that we remodel our . How do you guys like |
|
|
65:46 | Sugar? Good, right. All . Let's make, let's chocolate, |
|
|
65:51 | and sugar together. Yeah. I , that's just, that's just like |
|
|
65:55 | winner recipe, right? And you what? That's what we do every |
|
|
65:59 | we do sugar, you reward your and your brain says give me more |
|
|
66:02 | that and neural modulation, it gives , right? And it's modifying how |
|
|
66:09 | brain responds to simple signals about fuel your body because my brain does not |
|
|
66:15 | that when I put in other I mean, maybe a little bit |
|
|
66:18 | fat but not protein. I put in my body. It's like, |
|
|
66:20 | , whatever, you know, it's like OK, that's kind of |
|
|
66:24 | . When I put sugar, it's like just keep shoving it in. |
|
|
66:27 | we could just put it through the directly, we'd be happier. And |
|
|
66:32 | basically what Cola are now. neuromodulation isn't done. Ionotropic, |
|
|
66:40 | They use metabolic pathways primarily. So methods. So, g protein coupled |
|
|
66:46 | is a very common way. Neuromodulation place now to give you a sense |
|
|
66:52 | scale here, I've got this little and it shows you look we have |
|
|
66:57 | , very fast sorts of interactions between . This would be like within the |
|
|
67:01 | of one millisecond. And then we things that last a little bit longer |
|
|
67:05 | then even longer and then ultimately modulation these long term changes that are taking |
|
|
67:11 | . So they're trying to give you sense of scale. All right. |
|
|
67:15 | when you're talking about fast, you're talking about some sort of, of |
|
|
67:20 | aer or something that's basically opening up channel really quickly so that you get |
|
|
67:23 | response very quickly. But as you along to the slow transmission and |
|
|
67:29 | what you're doing is you're acting through some sort of metabotropic pathway. And |
|
|
67:36 | those words don't mean anything to, just kind of look at them for |
|
|
67:39 | second, ionotropic means working with right? Tropic means regulating. So |
|
|
67:45 | ion regulation. And so over here the left, that's an ionotropic |
|
|
67:51 | right? I'm releasing a neurotransmitter, neurotransmitter. In this case, Aceto |
|
|
67:55 | binds to a channel, it opens the channel. Now you have an |
|
|
68:00 | of sodium into the cell. So get a very quick, very rapid |
|
|
68:04 | . So an sp for example, a rapid response odds and it's very |
|
|
68:08 | and it's very short lived. All . But then there is the metabotropic |
|
|
68:13 | of interactions. And so here um actually doing Aceto Cole again. But |
|
|
68:17 | could imagine this could be glutamate, example, glutamate has a metabotropic as |
|
|
68:22 | as an ionotropic. But here you see Aceto cole goes and binds to |
|
|
68:25 | G protein coupled receptor. Now, of that is to open up a |
|
|
68:29 | . But do you think if I a G protein coupled receptor, there |
|
|
68:32 | be something downstream of that? So I'm gonna get a longer lived |
|
|
68:37 | and it's gonna be a little bit as well. And I throw this |
|
|
68:43 | here just again to kind of do little bit of a comparison here, |
|
|
68:48 | ? You can see that there are ways that I can open up |
|
|
68:53 | right? So here is the YAP , right? Here's my neurotransmitter binds |
|
|
68:57 | ion comes through here. I have neurotransmitter bind a G protein couple |
|
|
69:01 | I can activate it directly through the protein. So here I am opening |
|
|
69:06 | the channel or I can open through signaling cascade like cycle K MP, |
|
|
69:12 | on PK A PK through some sort mechanism, the phosphorylation or the opening |
|
|
69:18 | another channel. So you can imagine terms of time, fastest, |
|
|
69:23 | fastest, slowest and then you have . So different mechanisms of activating through |
|
|
69:38 | different types of receptors and different I mentioned glutamate having both a metabotropic |
|
|
69:47 | an isotropic. And I want to you an example of this sort of |
|
|
69:54 | . All right. So glutamate by produces an sp the way that it |
|
|
69:59 | this, it can act through a receptor which we're ignoring. So just |
|
|
70:03 | not going to look at that What it does is it acts through |
|
|
70:06 | ample receptor receptor. All right, is a form of ionotropic and you |
|
|
70:10 | it down here. So here is binds the receptor ions come in and |
|
|
70:15 | starting to see cell depolarized. you can see here here's an MD |
|
|
70:20 | receptor. This MD MN MD A is also capable of binding glutamate. |
|
|
70:27 | when it does, nothing happens, have to make modifications to that receptor |
|
|
70:32 | order for it to happen. And happens through the activity of the ample |
|
|
70:37 | . So what happens is is when um I think it's potassium basically, |
|
|
70:43 | the cell depolarizes, it causes MD to remove or release magnesium from its |
|
|
70:51 | , right? And so when magnesium basically what you've unblocked it. And |
|
|
70:55 | now when glutamate binds to the N A receptor, we've created a second |
|
|
71:01 | through which it can act put into . Over here, I have one |
|
|
71:09 | , one channel small response. After slight modification, I now have two |
|
|
71:15 | , bigger response. What is Neuromodulation? Oh And by the |
|
|
71:22 | we have another one, Kate, can be found up here. Glutamate |
|
|
71:27 | actually bind up to that and it tells the cell itself, hey, |
|
|
71:32 | ahead and release more neurotransmitter. So by itself can regulate its own cell |
|
|
71:41 | cause more glutamate to be released. . Yeah. So the question |
|
|
71:59 | does neuromodulation cause changes or result or be translated into modifications and behavior? |
|
|
72:05 | answer is yes. Now that would much kind of further downstream. So |
|
|
72:11 | other words, it wouldn't be like immediate thing. But you can imagine |
|
|
72:15 | you keep repeating something over and over over again, then the cell is |
|
|
72:21 | to start saying, oh, this how I'm being stimulated. So I'm |
|
|
72:25 | to change the way I respond to . And that's going to be here |
|
|
72:28 | just a, in a second. want to show you what that, |
|
|
72:33 | it is collectively. What we refer this as is plasticity. All |
|
|
72:38 | So we have what we refer to neuroplasticity and that neuroplasticity is us changing |
|
|
72:43 | response to the signals that our body our nervous system receives. No, |
|
|
72:52 | necessarily. And I'm, I'm just that's biochemistry and that sounds scary to |
|
|
72:59 | . I'm, I'm afraid the answers of course or no, it's |
|
|
73:04 | I don't know the answer to Right. It's OK to say, |
|
|
73:10 | don't know, sometimes can do All right, let's look at neuroplasticity |
|
|
73:15 | , very briefly here. All with regard to neuroplasticity, what you're |
|
|
73:20 | is you are changing your responsiveness in to a particular stimuli. And so |
|
|
73:26 | little example of plasticity is what your used said. Look, we have |
|
|
73:30 | example of low frequency stimulation and high stimulation, you can see it marked |
|
|
73:34 | here. So you can see the potentials are like this and then down |
|
|
73:41 | a lot faster. So when I'm low frequency, I'm going to release |
|
|
73:48 | a small amount of neurotransmitter. All . But in the case of high |
|
|
73:54 | , I'm not only releasing the neurotransmitter I normally release, but what I'm |
|
|
73:58 | going to do is I'm going to releasing a second neurotransmitter to give a |
|
|
74:02 | bigger response at the synapse. All . So I respond differently when I'm |
|
|
74:09 | differently is how this neuron is If I change the type of stimulus |
|
|
74:18 | do, then the cell is going respond differently. The early experiments that |
|
|
74:22 | were doing in plasticity, what they using sea slugs, right? So |
|
|
74:27 | about those cute little slug like right? And what they do is |
|
|
74:32 | reason we use them is because they big fat neurons that you can work |
|
|
74:36 | . And you know, back in sixties and fifties when they're working on |
|
|
74:39 | , they don't have micro instruments, only have big ones. And so |
|
|
74:42 | are easy to get a hold of you can stick them for all sorts |
|
|
74:45 | stuff, right? But one of things they were doing with the sea |
|
|
74:49 | is they were stimulating by taking a tiny rod. And what do slugs |
|
|
74:52 | snails have? You have those big ice stocks. Right. And then |
|
|
74:56 | they did was they would take a rod and they would tap the ice |
|
|
75:01 | . So, here you are, a cute little slug cruising out into |
|
|
75:04 | little tiny aquarium and someone comes up baps you on your eye. And |
|
|
75:09 | do you do if you're a sea ? Right. And then after a |
|
|
75:15 | you're like, I didn't like Well, I think it's ok and |
|
|
75:21 | it comes again and the, and do you do? And over time |
|
|
75:27 | keep hitting that and it becomes less to you. And eventually what you're |
|
|
75:32 | is you're like, this is how live now as someone sitting there bapping |
|
|
75:36 | on my eye stock, right? is a form of, of plasticity |
|
|
75:44 | habituation. All right, you have habituated to all sorts of things, |
|
|
75:50 | ? You have modulated or changed behavior a function of a constant stimuli around |
|
|
75:57 | , right? This is normal. is what your body does and you |
|
|
76:01 | kind of see what happens here is series of action potential is just kind |
|
|
76:04 | always going on. And the cell , you know what, I'm not |
|
|
76:09 | respond to each one of these action anymore. You're gonna really have to |
|
|
76:14 | me on in order to make this . And so what happens is your |
|
|
76:18 | rate decreases over time is what it basically showing you up there. All |
|
|
76:24 | , don't here they're showing you different of changes. So there's what we |
|
|
76:29 | to as facilitation. We just saw very, very quick, we have |
|
|
76:33 | , which is a little bit longer then potentiation, which is even longer |
|
|
76:36 | that. And so in here, just trying to show facilitation versus |
|
|
76:40 | You can see how you get this term response after a massive uh change |
|
|
76:46 | the signal. Whereas facilitation was just , OK, I'm responding to the |
|
|
76:50 | signals. But then, you I'm stopping, depression is, is |
|
|
76:54 | to habituation, but it is more line or opposite of facilitation. So |
|
|
77:01 | kind of a form of inhibition. why do we care? You |
|
|
77:10 | I mean, ultimately, I just you a bunch of words and you're |
|
|
77:12 | , so what, why do I about these different things? Because really |
|
|
77:18 | is how your body responds and your system responds to changes so that it |
|
|
77:25 | then govern how you behave and govern you respond to different sorts of |
|
|
77:33 | The thing is is when you this is what's going on, it's |
|
|
77:37 | and depression, we're increasing activity between cells or we're decreasing or we might |
|
|
77:46 | doing both simultaneously where if I'm the , I may potentiate here and depress |
|
|
77:52 | here and then change that relationship. so thoughts, ideas, behaviors, |
|
|
77:59 | are a function of the potentiation of that occurs my second movie and I |
|
|
78:07 | get the name, right? The Carrey. One spotless sunshine of the |
|
|
78:11 | mind or whatever it is, you , the real, you guys know |
|
|
78:14 | one I'm talking about but I never the title right? OK. Something |
|
|
78:20 | that. Eternal sunshine of the spotless . There it is just write it |
|
|
78:26 | . These are the movies you have see because reasons, in essence, |
|
|
78:29 | this movie, what happens Jim Carey this girl break up and the girl |
|
|
78:33 | to forget that she was ever in breakup or that she ever dated |
|
|
78:37 | And so there's a company that can in and erase memories and the way |
|
|
78:42 | they portray this is that memories are in individual neurons. And so the |
|
|
78:47 | is is that the the thought the is trying to escape the process of |
|
|
78:53 | and somehow Jim Carey knows that he's trying to be forgotten or something like |
|
|
78:58 | . It's a really interesting concept, it's completely false because that's not what |
|
|
79:02 | is. Memory is. The interactions multiple cells and the number of neurons |
|
|
79:08 | are involved and the firing that's happening between them. So when you experience |
|
|
79:14 | , you create this this pattern and pattern is what is reproduced. When |
|
|
79:18 | remember an idea, I know that's of weird. So for example, |
|
|
79:23 | of Statue of Liberty the first time learned Statue of Liberty, right? |
|
|
79:28 | you can now visualize, you can statue of Liberty, you created a |
|
|
79:33 | of interactions. And so that that is what is being reproduced so that |
|
|
79:38 | can remember that idea. And it's of these processes here, Tuesday, |
|
|
79:46 | have a test. Now why you say to that is because it means |
|
|
79:51 | class is a quarter over. You thought about it like that, |
|
|
79:56 | And then we get to move on some more interesting stuff after this because |
|
|
80:01 | now leveled the playing field and we're on the same page. So, |
|
|
80:04 | class on Tuesday, I'll see you Thursday. Go, get A's, |
|
|
80:09 | the weekend. It's not all about . I know you think it |
|
|
80:13 | but you got to have some fun . This. Uh-huh. Um, |
|
|
80:19 | then our, the |
|