© Distribution of this video is restricted by its owner
Transcript ×
Auto highlight
Font-size
00:04 This is Cellular Neuro Science lecture And we will talk a little bit

00:11 the origins, I would say of neuroscience uh for the rest of the

00:16 20 minutes or so in neuroscience. if you haven't taken my course

00:23 if you have taken, do you what I asked you to do?

00:26 said, look at the slide at very first lecture. I'm gonna look

00:28 the slide at the end of the . So the same way with those

00:32 haven't taken this course, just meditate this for a few seconds and see

00:36 you understand. But it's an activity billions of neurons surrounded by glial cells

00:44 form neuronal networks that communicate with each through specific connections or synopsis. And

00:52 trillions of these synopsis that are formed neurons informing the high central nervous

00:59 which is the brain with uh with , with the lobes, uh frontal

01:07 , parietal lobe, temporal lobe, lobes, and of course, the

01:13 stem and the spinal cord that goes the vertebrae. Of course textbook.

01:19 the same textbook that I use for section 4315. But it's a different

01:28 in particular. It's interesting that we through the beginning of this book and

01:34 we go to the end of this , we kind of uh leave some

01:37 in the middle and that's simply we cover a lot of information. And

01:42 a while I didn't even rely on book for the cellular neuroscience course,

01:46 they have improved it, updated it I found it pretty suitable for what

01:51 want to cover this semester. Neuroscience comes in many different levels of

01:57 We're going to focus on cellular At the very beginning, our understanding

02:07 different functions of the brain came from loss of function studies. So this

02:14 Doctor Paul Broca, the uh for , so we speak with the left

02:23 . Now, the reason why he that is because he had a patient

02:30 that patient had damage to this which is now called the Brocas area

02:40 the left hemisphere. And patients that damage to that specific area, which

02:46 called brokers area had expressive aphasia, aphasia is an ability to convey thoughts

02:54 speech or writing. So it affects motor ability because Broca area here shown

03:02 in blue is very closely located to primary motor cortex. So it affects

03:11 ability to speak or write. And several brains by Doctor Broer were studied

03:20 had damage to the same area and had expressive aphasia. There was also

03:27 discovery of localization of specific brain Uh Shortly after Doctor Vernet discovered that

03:35 damage to an area that is located in the temporal lobe and temporal lobe

03:43 into the parietal lobes area. damage to Vernis area results in receptive

03:52 which is a difficulty to understand uh or written language. So expressive is

04:01 to express yourself or subject is difficulty receive that language. And it was

04:08 clear that there are now two areas the brain Broca area responsible for expression

04:17 Vernik area responsible for receptive and reception this speech. But it came from

04:24 of function studies and it still is a gross anatomical level, not like

04:31 , gross but gross anatomy and gross level. So big holes in the

04:39 . Uh There's also another form of called anomic amnesia, aphasia, which

04:47 the least severe form of aphasia. I think that I definitely suffer from

04:53 after long weekends especially and having too fun and global aphasia, which can

04:59 from severe and extensive damage to the and typically damaging large areas of the

05:08 involving many many areas that process the and produce the language. And that

05:17 another lesson from the slide is there one area responsible for language. There

05:24 two areas, there are many areas are interconnected with their own specific functions

05:32 language such as understanding it or expressing um and such now, one of

05:43 most famous patients in Neurosciences concerns the of function as P GAUGE. He

05:51 a massive accident in 1848 where this strip that he's holding that is shown

05:59 the picture, this metal strip penetrated the bottom of the skull, through

06:05 eye and exited out at the top the skull just like it was shown

06:10 this diagram. And this is his skull in the FG skull. The

06:16 and it's such a severe trauma. lot of people observing what happened,

06:22 was a a fault explosive device that off, that set off this uh

06:29 metal bar into his brain. Many that he was probably going to die

06:36 that he's not going to recover. he recovers that he's probably gonna not

06:42 able to walk or talk or be vegetable. And in fact, he

06:46 not, he came back and he his job back. He could

06:51 he could walk, he could you couldn't see with one eye,

06:54 his behavior was awful and uh he aggressive, he couldn't control himself specifically

07:04 aggression. And we now understood that large areas of the brain, it

07:10 matter if you lose a large area the brain is what those parts or

07:14 of the brain are responsible for. can lose much smaller area of the

07:19 and not being able to see At all very small area of the

07:23 or you can have sustained this damage that to large area of the frontal

07:30 . But seemingly almost have all of functions except for behavioral cognitive aggression,

07:41 functions that get compromised as a consequence the damage to the frontal lobe.

07:47 still is a loss of function. still is on the gross anatomical

07:53 And cellular neuroscience starts when it starts we can see the cells. So

08:01 comes the cell theory and Neuroscience is neuron doctrine. In order to see

08:09 , you need tools to see these and these tools are microscopes and only

08:15 the 19th century and really talk about . Uh end of the 19th century

08:23 when you have microscopes that have enough to see individual cells. But the

08:35 presents a problem because if you just the brain out or if you cut

08:40 brain into slices, you really don't anything, it's translucent. And therefore

08:48 debate enrages in the field. The of reticular theory argue that all of

08:56 brain, all of the neurons are of one humongous structure envelope by the

09:04 cytoplasmic membrane. Therefore, they have continuity and they're all this massive multi

09:12 structure that's all together CSI. And the opposing side, you have the

09:21 doctrine. So Camello Golgi is a of the reticular theory, his student

09:27 Cajal, which is probably one of most famous neuroscientists in the world

09:33 undoubtedly in Spain Ramona Cajal is a of the neuron doctrine. And so

09:39 Sir Charles Sherrington that comes into picture on, uh neuron doctrine argued that

09:47 , this is not big clump surrounded one membrane. In fact that these

09:52 individual discrete unit cells or later called that have their own membrane surrounding each

10:02 individual discrete cells. And in order really understand the cells and to visualize

10:11 cells in the brain, we needed . So Camelio Golgi invented the Golgi

10:18 . Ramona Cajal was a student and Cajal used the Gogi stain under microscope

10:26 perform these beautiful reconstructions. This is two eyes and the nerves. And

10:33 can see that he drew the projections the eyes, some that remain on

10:39 same side of so lateral others coming the nasal part of the retina here

10:45 those would be in the middle are crossing over through the optics.

10:52 he depicted these cells that are called cells in the cerebellum that to this

10:59 are known to contain hundreds of thousands synopsis or points of contact onto

11:08 Uh This is a reconstruction of the of the brain that will study in

11:14 detail called the hippocampus. And he the cell or reconstructed the cell.

11:21 these are not drawings that are called . So Golgi stain gets picked up

11:27 a fraction of neurons And when you the Golgi stain, those neurons will

11:34 all of their processes. So we'll the dendrites the dendritic projections. This

11:41 the dendritic tree, the camus and the axons axons is where you will

11:47 the synaptic transmission, take place of between the neurons. And even so

11:55 this stain, uh you can see lot of details, you can even

11:59 dendritic spines using Golgi stain and Golgi is still actually used to study some

12:05 the dendritic spine anatomy, static But you cannot uh visualize those

12:13 the space between neurons and how they to each other is very small.

12:17 so Goji probably takes these drawings and says, so what they're still all

12:23 , even if you're showing me these tree that does not agree with

12:28 How now uh in addition to the stain, there is another stain that

12:35 our understanding of the anatomy of the . And that is the Niel stain

12:41 contrast to Golgi stain, Nel stain picked up by all of the

12:47 all neurons and all gluon. So stain gets picked up only by a

12:52 of neurons and it will expose their brilliantly, all of the processes.

12:59 stain will get picked up by all the neurons and glia and where you

13:03 darker bands, this indicates higher densities higher presence packing densities of certain

13:12 But Nissel stain does not reveal all precise morphology of neurons, the dendritic

13:20 axonal projections. Instead, it is great tool to describe the architecture.

13:27 overall what we call cyto architecture of brain cyto architecture is if you think

13:35 the Celsus bricks, how they how densely are they stacked? Which

13:41 are they pointing in that stack? so there are these methods that architectonic

13:48 um that with the help of this , you get picked up by RN

13:55 or polar ribosomes uh mostly near the right Irish expression of those would be

14:02 the SOMA exposing all of them. um not a great tool for morphology

14:08 individual neurons, but a great tool cytoarchitecture of neurons uh and neuronal

14:15 the brain as a whole. So and Brodman uses Nissel stain and comes

14:22 with broad areas of the brain where basically describes the cyto architecture of different

14:29 in different parts of the brain. standard light microscope, if we look

14:40 the standard light microscope, even if can go to 0.1 microns micrometers.

14:50 let's start with this one centimeter is . How many micro meters? One

15:05 ? 10,000? How about one millimeter have my phone or maybe a

15:18 So one it's like 1,000,000 micro uh . So then centimeter is 10,000.

15:32 . Oh All right. So this is 0.1 micrometer. What is that

15:46 ? This Yeah. So that's the microscope. Maybe you can push it

16:01 but you cannot, you cannot see uh or greater resolution, higher resolution

16:10 oops runaway mouse uh the space between two synapses. So the space where

16:23 neuron is going to release the chemical and the next neuron that has receptors

16:32 going to receive this neurotransmitter. This here is 20 nanometers in chemical

16:41 So now you cannot visualize, you have enough of resolution. If you're

16:48 this is 20 nanometers and the resolution 100 nano meters, then no,

16:54 will just see uh one point. electron microscopes which come about in the

17:03 century, middle of the 20th century down to 0.1 nano be dis

17:10 So those are sufficient enough to to visualize the synopsis that space actually.

17:23 to visualize neurons, we don't need these days, we can still use

17:30 , we still use a lot of . There's a whole chapter of stains

17:36 sigma which that you can order for or different molecular stains, different cytoplasmic

17:43 skeletal elements, anything you want. in electrophysiology and we'll talk about

17:50 of course, quite a bit in , we use infrared microscopy.

17:57 Ir so if you place a slice the brain and you expose it to

18:03 wavelength of light and in the you have the Ir Camera, infrared

18:10 , you can visualize individual neurons to individual neurons. Again, you don't

18:16 that resolution of a single synapse. if you're talking about the scales,

18:22 synapse is 20 nanometer. This is C OK. The cell is typically

18:33 10 micrometers of the the neuron. can be smaller, can be large

18:40 it can be very large, can very small in some instances. But

18:43 the most part, it's about 10 in di so can visualize these neurons

18:50 . We can target them with the tip of the microelectrode that reports

18:58 this cell or another cell. So tip of the microelectrode is typically one

19:05 to one micrometer. So we can this technique, infrared microscopy or infrared

19:15 . Uh you're not looking into the objectives here. This infrared camera is

19:21 to a monitor and it shows you picture we can't see an infrared

19:25 But it it shows it on the , we can see it and we

19:30 now perform electrophysiological recordings by targeting the of interest with the micro electrons.

19:38 is important uh aspect of neurons, spines that cover dendrites. These are

19:49 , those protrusions come in different You can see that these dendritic spines

19:56 contain PSD stands for den for dendrite , for cross synaptic density. They

20:02 contain a lot of receptors and they're to the presynaptic terminal. So this

20:09 the presynaptic terminal that contains these round vesicles that will contain neurotrans livers.

20:17 most of the points of contact that onto neurons happen. The synopsis are

20:23 on to these dendritic spines from axons the dendritic spines. So, there

20:29 different variations and exceptions of how the and where they're formed, it can

20:34 even formed between two dendrites. But the most part, they are a

20:41 somatic or axodendritic and dendritic spines can for a lot of points of contact

20:47 that dendritic uh tree. All So I think we're almost out of

20:56 , but I would like to draw attention to the fact that in modern

21:02 neuroscience, we can visualize individual molecules visualize individual dendritic spines. We can

21:10 activity in single cells and networks of . And also in the clinical

21:16 we can visualize the brain network This is using positives on emission tomography

21:22 looking at the words will engage certain of the brain. Speaking of the

21:27 , another part of the brain listening the words, another distinct oh brain

21:32 will be produced because it shows the of neurons in that area and then

21:38 of the words good. There are is this slide, there's three more

21:46 that I'm not going to cover I'm gonna leave it here. But

21:50 will mention this because a big problem neuroscience and in cellular neuroscience has been

21:59 deep can you observe things even in imaging? When we talk about positron

22:05 tomography or even F MRI functional Magnetic Symmetry. It's like how deep can

22:12 see how deep can you record real ? So you can see pretty deep

22:17 these, with these techniques pet scan MRI with electrophysiology, you can record

22:24 deep as you can penetrate inside the . So what typically is on the

22:31 ? And a lot of these techniques we have non invasive techniques that can

22:35 activity deep will not do it on cell level, we'll do it on

22:40 network level. And so that's where experimental neuroscience comes comes into play again

22:47 techniques like two photon imaging analysis, photon imaging. This is done in

22:54 imaging of neurons imaging of their activity within this is cortex and you can

23:03 from 0 to 1000 micrometers deep. is almost the entire depth of the

23:10 cortex. So this is another thing we're after we're after spatial resolution.

23:17 we image individual cell synopsis? How connect molecules where after after death,

23:24 deep can we image these things? deep can we understand this normal

23:29 not just on the surface of electrodes also using microscopy techniques like three photon

23:37 . All right. So I'm gonna here when we come back, we'll

23:39 a little bit about artificial intelligence and it uh it can be used potentially

23:47 neurological disorders. This is another point interest for some of you that mentioned

23:52 you're interested in what happens in And I didn't go over the third

23:59 of the course in the syllabus. our third section of the course is

24:05 heavily focused on several neurological disorders uh we will instead of spending 1015

24:12 we will spend a whole hour and minutes or in case of epilepsy,

24:17 three hours talking about epilepsy and understanding mechanisms, cellular mechanisms and uh some

24:24 the clinical aspects like symptomology as well therapy for, for these disorders.

24:30 . Thank you very much. Have great weekend this weekend. All

24:34 And I'll see everyone here on

-
+