© Distribution of this video is restricted by its owner
00:04 | This is Cellular Neuro Science lecture And we will talk a little bit |
|
|
00:11 | the origins, I would say of neuroscience uh for the rest of the |
|
|
00:16 | 20 minutes or so in neuroscience. if you haven't taken my course |
|
|
00:23 | if you have taken, do you what I asked you to do? |
|
|
00:26 | said, look at the slide at very first lecture. I'm gonna look |
|
|
00:28 | the slide at the end of the . So the same way with those |
|
|
00:32 | haven't taken this course, just meditate this for a few seconds and see |
|
|
00:36 | you understand. But it's an activity billions of neurons surrounded by glial cells |
|
|
00:44 | form neuronal networks that communicate with each through specific connections or synopsis. And |
|
|
00:52 | trillions of these synopsis that are formed neurons informing the high central nervous |
|
|
00:59 | which is the brain with uh with , with the lobes, uh frontal |
|
|
01:07 | , parietal lobe, temporal lobe, lobes, and of course, the |
|
|
01:13 | stem and the spinal cord that goes the vertebrae. Of course textbook. |
|
|
01:19 | the same textbook that I use for section 4315. But it's a different |
|
|
01:28 | in particular. It's interesting that we through the beginning of this book and |
|
|
01:34 | we go to the end of this , we kind of uh leave some |
|
|
01:37 | in the middle and that's simply we cover a lot of information. And |
|
|
01:42 | a while I didn't even rely on book for the cellular neuroscience course, |
|
|
01:46 | they have improved it, updated it I found it pretty suitable for what |
|
|
01:51 | want to cover this semester. Neuroscience comes in many different levels of |
|
|
01:57 | We're going to focus on cellular At the very beginning, our understanding |
|
|
02:07 | different functions of the brain came from loss of function studies. So this |
|
|
02:14 | Doctor Paul Broca, the uh for , so we speak with the left |
|
|
02:23 | . Now, the reason why he that is because he had a patient |
|
|
02:30 | that patient had damage to this which is now called the Brocas area |
|
|
02:40 | the left hemisphere. And patients that damage to that specific area, which |
|
|
02:46 | called brokers area had expressive aphasia, aphasia is an ability to convey thoughts |
|
|
02:54 | speech or writing. So it affects motor ability because Broca area here shown |
|
|
03:02 | in blue is very closely located to primary motor cortex. So it affects |
|
|
03:11 | ability to speak or write. And several brains by Doctor Broer were studied |
|
|
03:20 | had damage to the same area and had expressive aphasia. There was also |
|
|
03:27 | discovery of localization of specific brain Uh Shortly after Doctor Vernet discovered that |
|
|
03:35 | damage to an area that is located in the temporal lobe and temporal lobe |
|
|
03:43 | into the parietal lobes area. damage to Vernis area results in receptive |
|
|
03:52 | which is a difficulty to understand uh or written language. So expressive is |
|
|
04:01 | to express yourself or subject is difficulty receive that language. And it was |
|
|
04:08 | clear that there are now two areas the brain Broca area responsible for expression |
|
|
04:17 | Vernik area responsible for receptive and reception this speech. But it came from |
|
|
04:24 | of function studies and it still is a gross anatomical level, not like |
|
|
04:31 | , gross but gross anatomy and gross level. So big holes in the |
|
|
04:39 | . Uh There's also another form of called anomic amnesia, aphasia, which |
|
|
04:47 | the least severe form of aphasia. I think that I definitely suffer from |
|
|
04:53 | after long weekends especially and having too fun and global aphasia, which can |
|
|
04:59 | from severe and extensive damage to the and typically damaging large areas of the |
|
|
05:08 | involving many many areas that process the and produce the language. And that |
|
|
05:17 | another lesson from the slide is there one area responsible for language. There |
|
|
05:24 | two areas, there are many areas are interconnected with their own specific functions |
|
|
05:32 | language such as understanding it or expressing um and such now, one of |
|
|
05:43 | most famous patients in Neurosciences concerns the of function as P GAUGE. He |
|
|
05:51 | a massive accident in 1848 where this strip that he's holding that is shown |
|
|
05:59 | the picture, this metal strip penetrated the bottom of the skull, through |
|
|
06:05 | eye and exited out at the top the skull just like it was shown |
|
|
06:10 | this diagram. And this is his skull in the FG skull. The |
|
|
06:16 | and it's such a severe trauma. lot of people observing what happened, |
|
|
06:22 | was a a fault explosive device that off, that set off this uh |
|
|
06:29 | metal bar into his brain. Many that he was probably going to die |
|
|
06:36 | that he's not going to recover. he recovers that he's probably gonna not |
|
|
06:42 | able to walk or talk or be vegetable. And in fact, he |
|
|
06:46 | not, he came back and he his job back. He could |
|
|
06:51 | he could walk, he could you couldn't see with one eye, |
|
|
06:54 | his behavior was awful and uh he aggressive, he couldn't control himself specifically |
|
|
07:04 | aggression. And we now understood that large areas of the brain, it |
|
|
07:10 | matter if you lose a large area the brain is what those parts or |
|
|
07:14 | of the brain are responsible for. can lose much smaller area of the |
|
|
07:19 | and not being able to see At all very small area of the |
|
|
07:23 | or you can have sustained this damage that to large area of the frontal |
|
|
07:30 | . But seemingly almost have all of functions except for behavioral cognitive aggression, |
|
|
07:41 | functions that get compromised as a consequence the damage to the frontal lobe. |
|
|
07:47 | still is a loss of function. still is on the gross anatomical |
|
|
07:53 | And cellular neuroscience starts when it starts we can see the cells. So |
|
|
08:01 | comes the cell theory and Neuroscience is neuron doctrine. In order to see |
|
|
08:09 | , you need tools to see these and these tools are microscopes and only |
|
|
08:15 | the 19th century and really talk about . Uh end of the 19th century |
|
|
08:23 | when you have microscopes that have enough to see individual cells. But the |
|
|
08:35 | presents a problem because if you just the brain out or if you cut |
|
|
08:40 | brain into slices, you really don't anything, it's translucent. And therefore |
|
|
08:48 | debate enrages in the field. The of reticular theory argue that all of |
|
|
08:56 | brain, all of the neurons are of one humongous structure envelope by the |
|
|
09:04 | cytoplasmic membrane. Therefore, they have continuity and they're all this massive multi |
|
|
09:12 | structure that's all together CSI. And the opposing side, you have the |
|
|
09:21 | doctrine. So Camello Golgi is a of the reticular theory, his student |
|
|
09:27 | Cajal, which is probably one of most famous neuroscientists in the world |
|
|
09:33 | undoubtedly in Spain Ramona Cajal is a of the neuron doctrine. And so |
|
|
09:39 | Sir Charles Sherrington that comes into picture on, uh neuron doctrine argued that |
|
|
09:47 | , this is not big clump surrounded one membrane. In fact that these |
|
|
09:52 | individual discrete unit cells or later called that have their own membrane surrounding each |
|
|
10:02 | individual discrete cells. And in order really understand the cells and to visualize |
|
|
10:11 | cells in the brain, we needed . So Camelio Golgi invented the Golgi |
|
|
10:18 | . Ramona Cajal was a student and Cajal used the Gogi stain under microscope |
|
|
10:26 | perform these beautiful reconstructions. This is two eyes and the nerves. And |
|
|
10:33 | can see that he drew the projections the eyes, some that remain on |
|
|
10:39 | same side of so lateral others coming the nasal part of the retina here |
|
|
10:45 | those would be in the middle are crossing over through the optics. |
|
|
10:52 | he depicted these cells that are called cells in the cerebellum that to this |
|
|
10:59 | are known to contain hundreds of thousands synopsis or points of contact onto |
|
|
11:08 | Uh This is a reconstruction of the of the brain that will study in |
|
|
11:14 | detail called the hippocampus. And he the cell or reconstructed the cell. |
|
|
11:21 | these are not drawings that are called . So Golgi stain gets picked up |
|
|
11:27 | a fraction of neurons And when you the Golgi stain, those neurons will |
|
|
11:34 | all of their processes. So we'll the dendrites the dendritic projections. This |
|
|
11:41 | the dendritic tree, the camus and the axons axons is where you will |
|
|
11:47 | the synaptic transmission, take place of between the neurons. And even so |
|
|
11:55 | this stain, uh you can see lot of details, you can even |
|
|
11:59 | dendritic spines using Golgi stain and Golgi is still actually used to study some |
|
|
12:05 | the dendritic spine anatomy, static But you cannot uh visualize those |
|
|
12:13 | the space between neurons and how they to each other is very small. |
|
|
12:17 | so Goji probably takes these drawings and says, so what they're still all |
|
|
12:23 | , even if you're showing me these tree that does not agree with |
|
|
12:28 | How now uh in addition to the stain, there is another stain that |
|
|
12:35 | our understanding of the anatomy of the . And that is the Niel stain |
|
|
12:41 | contrast to Golgi stain, Nel stain picked up by all of the |
|
|
12:47 | all neurons and all gluon. So stain gets picked up only by a |
|
|
12:52 | of neurons and it will expose their brilliantly, all of the processes. |
|
|
12:59 | stain will get picked up by all the neurons and glia and where you |
|
|
13:03 | darker bands, this indicates higher densities higher presence packing densities of certain |
|
|
13:12 | But Nissel stain does not reveal all precise morphology of neurons, the dendritic |
|
|
13:20 | axonal projections. Instead, it is great tool to describe the architecture. |
|
|
13:27 | overall what we call cyto architecture of brain cyto architecture is if you think |
|
|
13:35 | the Celsus bricks, how they how densely are they stacked? Which |
|
|
13:41 | are they pointing in that stack? so there are these methods that architectonic |
|
|
13:48 | um that with the help of this , you get picked up by RN |
|
|
13:55 | or polar ribosomes uh mostly near the right Irish expression of those would be |
|
|
14:02 | the SOMA exposing all of them. um not a great tool for morphology |
|
|
14:08 | individual neurons, but a great tool cytoarchitecture of neurons uh and neuronal |
|
|
14:15 | the brain as a whole. So and Brodman uses Nissel stain and comes |
|
|
14:22 | with broad areas of the brain where basically describes the cyto architecture of different |
|
|
14:29 | in different parts of the brain. standard light microscope, if we look |
|
|
14:40 | the standard light microscope, even if can go to 0.1 microns micrometers. |
|
|
14:50 | let's start with this one centimeter is . How many micro meters? One |
|
|
15:05 | ? 10,000? How about one millimeter have my phone or maybe a |
|
|
15:18 | So one it's like 1,000,000 micro uh . So then centimeter is 10,000. |
|
|
15:32 | . Oh All right. So this is 0.1 micrometer. What is that |
|
|
15:46 | ? This Yeah. So that's the microscope. Maybe you can push it |
|
|
16:01 | but you cannot, you cannot see uh or greater resolution, higher resolution |
|
|
16:10 | oops runaway mouse uh the space between two synapses. So the space where |
|
|
16:23 | neuron is going to release the chemical and the next neuron that has receptors |
|
|
16:32 | going to receive this neurotransmitter. This here is 20 nanometers in chemical |
|
|
16:41 | So now you cannot visualize, you have enough of resolution. If you're |
|
|
16:48 | this is 20 nanometers and the resolution 100 nano meters, then no, |
|
|
16:54 | will just see uh one point. electron microscopes which come about in the |
|
|
17:03 | century, middle of the 20th century down to 0.1 nano be dis |
|
|
17:10 | So those are sufficient enough to to visualize the synopsis that space actually. |
|
|
17:23 | to visualize neurons, we don't need these days, we can still use |
|
|
17:30 | , we still use a lot of . There's a whole chapter of stains |
|
|
17:36 | sigma which that you can order for or different molecular stains, different cytoplasmic |
|
|
17:43 | skeletal elements, anything you want. in electrophysiology and we'll talk about |
|
|
17:50 | of course, quite a bit in , we use infrared microscopy. |
|
|
17:57 | Ir so if you place a slice the brain and you expose it to |
|
|
18:03 | wavelength of light and in the you have the Ir Camera, infrared |
|
|
18:10 | , you can visualize individual neurons to individual neurons. Again, you don't |
|
|
18:16 | that resolution of a single synapse. if you're talking about the scales, |
|
|
18:22 | synapse is 20 nanometer. This is C OK. The cell is typically |
|
|
18:33 | 10 micrometers of the the neuron. can be smaller, can be large |
|
|
18:40 | it can be very large, can very small in some instances. But |
|
|
18:43 | the most part, it's about 10 in di so can visualize these neurons |
|
|
18:50 | . We can target them with the tip of the microelectrode that reports |
|
|
18:58 | this cell or another cell. So tip of the microelectrode is typically one |
|
|
19:05 | to one micrometer. So we can this technique, infrared microscopy or infrared |
|
|
19:15 | . Uh you're not looking into the objectives here. This infrared camera is |
|
|
19:21 | to a monitor and it shows you picture we can't see an infrared |
|
|
19:25 | But it it shows it on the , we can see it and we |
|
|
19:30 | now perform electrophysiological recordings by targeting the of interest with the micro electrons. |
|
|
19:38 | is important uh aspect of neurons, spines that cover dendrites. These are |
|
|
19:49 | , those protrusions come in different You can see that these dendritic spines |
|
|
19:56 | contain PSD stands for den for dendrite , for cross synaptic density. They |
|
|
20:02 | contain a lot of receptors and they're to the presynaptic terminal. So this |
|
|
20:09 | the presynaptic terminal that contains these round vesicles that will contain neurotrans livers. |
|
|
20:17 | most of the points of contact that onto neurons happen. The synopsis are |
|
|
20:23 | on to these dendritic spines from axons the dendritic spines. So, there |
|
|
20:29 | different variations and exceptions of how the and where they're formed, it can |
|
|
20:34 | even formed between two dendrites. But the most part, they are a |
|
|
20:41 | somatic or axodendritic and dendritic spines can for a lot of points of contact |
|
|
20:47 | that dendritic uh tree. All So I think we're almost out of |
|
|
20:56 | , but I would like to draw attention to the fact that in modern |
|
|
21:02 | neuroscience, we can visualize individual molecules visualize individual dendritic spines. We can |
|
|
21:10 | activity in single cells and networks of . And also in the clinical |
|
|
21:16 | we can visualize the brain network This is using positives on emission tomography |
|
|
21:22 | looking at the words will engage certain of the brain. Speaking of the |
|
|
21:27 | , another part of the brain listening the words, another distinct oh brain |
|
|
21:32 | will be produced because it shows the of neurons in that area and then |
|
|
21:38 | of the words good. There are is this slide, there's three more |
|
|
21:46 | that I'm not going to cover I'm gonna leave it here. But |
|
|
21:50 | will mention this because a big problem neuroscience and in cellular neuroscience has been |
|
|
21:59 | deep can you observe things even in imaging? When we talk about positron |
|
|
22:05 | tomography or even F MRI functional Magnetic Symmetry. It's like how deep can |
|
|
22:12 | see how deep can you record real ? So you can see pretty deep |
|
|
22:17 | these, with these techniques pet scan MRI with electrophysiology, you can record |
|
|
22:24 | deep as you can penetrate inside the . So what typically is on the |
|
|
22:31 | ? And a lot of these techniques we have non invasive techniques that can |
|
|
22:35 | activity deep will not do it on cell level, we'll do it on |
|
|
22:40 | network level. And so that's where experimental neuroscience comes comes into play again |
|
|
22:47 | techniques like two photon imaging analysis, photon imaging. This is done in |
|
|
22:54 | imaging of neurons imaging of their activity within this is cortex and you can |
|
|
23:03 | from 0 to 1000 micrometers deep. is almost the entire depth of the |
|
|
23:10 | cortex. So this is another thing we're after we're after spatial resolution. |
|
|
23:17 | we image individual cell synopsis? How connect molecules where after after death, |
|
|
23:24 | deep can we image these things? deep can we understand this normal |
|
|
23:29 | not just on the surface of electrodes also using microscopy techniques like three photon |
|
|
23:37 | . All right. So I'm gonna here when we come back, we'll |
|
|
23:39 | a little bit about artificial intelligence and it uh it can be used potentially |
|
|
23:47 | neurological disorders. This is another point interest for some of you that mentioned |
|
|
23:52 | you're interested in what happens in And I didn't go over the third |
|
|
23:59 | of the course in the syllabus. our third section of the course is |
|
|
24:05 | heavily focused on several neurological disorders uh we will instead of spending 1015 |
|
|
24:12 | we will spend a whole hour and minutes or in case of epilepsy, |
|
|
24:17 | three hours talking about epilepsy and understanding mechanisms, cellular mechanisms and uh some |
|
|
24:24 | the clinical aspects like symptomology as well therapy for, for these disorders. |
|
|
24:30 | . Thank you very much. Have great weekend this weekend. All |
|
|
24:34 | And I'll see everyone here on |
|