© Distribution of this video is restricted by its owner
00:07 | This is lecture four of Neuroscience and important things that we talked about that |
|
|
00:12 | continue talking about today where the organelles these cells. And in particular, |
|
|
00:20 | spend a little bit of time talking the cyto skeletal elements. And we |
|
|
00:24 | that there are three major different sub of cyto skeletal elements, microtubules. |
|
|
00:32 | let me zoom in. So everybody see better the microtubules, neurofilament and |
|
|
00:40 | and micro filaments. We discussed as smallest cyto skeletal elements also comprise of |
|
|
00:47 | smallest molecules. And we talked about ability of these cyto skeletal elements to |
|
|
00:54 | themselves and reorganize themselves, sort of a structural support lattice, like support |
|
|
01:00 | the overall structure of the cell, also for the outer membrane boundaries of |
|
|
01:05 | cell. And we spoke about how elements have the ability to polymerize themselves |
|
|
01:10 | longer chains and depolymerize and get broken into shorter chains. You can think |
|
|
01:17 | it as different structural elements by which can reconfigure a house uh using certain |
|
|
01:23 | of these elements. Now, we that the act in elements because they're |
|
|
01:28 | smallest ones are also gonna be located the outer boundaries of the south as |
|
|
01:33 | indicated by this label. And the labeled here in yellow will be mostly |
|
|
01:41 | the nuclear region. But also you see these what we call micro tullar |
|
|
01:47 | extending into the processes and essentially being for external transport of vesicles of |
|
|
01:56 | Um We further talked about axon that has pretty complex anatomy and different aspects |
|
|
02:04 | this anatomy talked about how the external . So once the goods get transported |
|
|
02:10 | , there's certain things that live there in the external uh in this uh |
|
|
02:15 | uh terminal, external terminal. And have mitochondria, you have vesicles filled |
|
|
02:20 | neurotransmitters. They confused presynaptic, you exocytosis. On the other side, |
|
|
02:26 | have postsynaptic dendrites or postsynaptic dendritic which is filled with postsynaptic receptors. |
|
|
02:33 | the chemicals from the vesicles will bind these possy nic receptors causing a possy |
|
|
02:38 | effect. So, we discussed also presynaptic versus the possy. And we |
|
|
02:44 | about how you can use different tracers even viruses in order to understand the |
|
|
02:53 | where the projections where the processes from . So must extend to how potentially |
|
|
02:58 | networks interconnect with each other. There also dyes that are called transsynaptic dyes |
|
|
03:03 | means that they're going to jump between and between synopsis. So you can |
|
|
03:09 | it across multiple connected synopsis. Uh we highlighted horseradish peroxidase as a, |
|
|
03:16 | a guy that you inject. And talked about herpes virus and rabies virus |
|
|
03:20 | are capable of this retrograde transport, from the periphery back into the SOMA |
|
|
03:27 | the anterograde transport, which was from SOMA into the peripheral regions. So |
|
|
03:35 | ended here on this slide last lecture our next slide begins uh our discussions |
|
|
03:46 | , well, it's not really the neurological disorder because we already have talked |
|
|
03:53 | epilepsy. And what we saw uh regards to epilepsy, is that fine |
|
|
04:01 | . You remember fas gauge, he traumatic brain injury and there was a |
|
|
04:06 | bit of discussion about traumatic brain injuries my other section. And re |
|
|
04:11 | I said that traumatic brain injuries can in other diseases. So in this |
|
|
04:19 | , we'll talk about another disease is and we'll talk about epilepsy, co |
|
|
04:25 | . We talked about epilepsy with regard traumatic brain injury that NAS gauge suffered |
|
|
04:31 | brain injury. However, he developed and actually passed not because of that |
|
|
04:38 | , but because he had status epileptic he had this massive uncontrollable seizure that |
|
|
04:44 | his death. And typically you have if you have epilepsy, which means |
|
|
04:49 | have repeated seizures throughout. We talked how following traumatic brain injury, epilepsy |
|
|
04:56 | a comorbidity may come about days weeks later, but sometimes years |
|
|
05:03 | And it's uh really interesting and on radio and on the news, you'll |
|
|
05:08 | that one of the US bases was by a drone and three of our |
|
|
05:14 | were killed and 25 were injured just . And the ones that were injured |
|
|
05:22 | , it's mentioned in the news from brain injuries. And so there are |
|
|
05:26 | types of traumatic brain injuries, brain or traumatic brain injuries that will cause |
|
|
05:32 | metal bar to penetrate through your A bullet to go a shrapnel and |
|
|
05:37 | are also brain injuries from being near explosive device, not even impacted |
|
|
05:45 | but a whiplash, a pressure wave other consequences. So this is really |
|
|
05:53 | because it's also traumatic brain injuries are related to epilepsy. They're also related |
|
|
05:59 | Alzheimer's disease and the condition that we will mention called chronic traumatic encephalopathy. |
|
|
06:06 | that is a condition that quite a contact athletes or football players especially may |
|
|
06:14 | if they have multiple repeated concussions and multiple repeated concussions are going to lead |
|
|
06:20 | CTE chronic traumatic encephalopathy, which shares lot of pathological hallmarks with Alzheimer's |
|
|
06:28 | I'm not saying that the main cause Alzheimer's disease is traumatic drainage. |
|
|
06:35 | it is correlated. A lot of things are intertwined through some of the |
|
|
06:40 | network or semi cellular mechanisms and cellular . But let's talk about more specifically |
|
|
06:48 | Alzheimer's disease here. When you think Alzheimer's disease, what comes to mind |
|
|
06:55 | getting stuff memory loss is that, that all? So that's what |
|
|
07:05 | most of you know, that's what call the symptom of the disease, |
|
|
07:11 | ? Because when somebody's forgetting stuff, don't see what's going on in the |
|
|
07:15 | and have a sample or spinal fluid their blood. So you don't know |
|
|
07:20 | somebody comes in and has typically the of Alzheimer's is 55 years of age |
|
|
07:26 | older. And the patients with Alzheimer's and early loss of Alzheimer's disease will |
|
|
07:33 | developing memory loss, starting with a term memory loss. So they're usually |
|
|
07:39 | members will start complaining. It's how many times do I have to |
|
|
07:42 | you make a sandwich for me? know, you keep asking what am |
|
|
07:45 | supposed to do? So typically it's short term memory loss, not remembering |
|
|
07:50 | just happened. And then if the , uh le gets left unchecked and |
|
|
07:59 | , currently there is no cure for disease. It is a disease that's |
|
|
08:08 | a part of the normal age. a terminal individuals died from Alzheimer's |
|
|
08:16 | And if these symptoms progress and it for each person, how fast symptomology |
|
|
08:24 | progress. That means it's getting worse the disease is progressing in pathology |
|
|
08:29 | that means it's getting worse in the sense. So from short term memory |
|
|
08:35 | to long term memory loss and typically progression of Alzheimer's disease, patients may |
|
|
08:42 | their family members for quite a while then they will actually start forgetting their |
|
|
08:47 | members and the names of their own and who they are. Now. |
|
|
08:52 | and, and apart from the symptomology memory loss, there's a lot of |
|
|
08:58 | things that are um associated as symptoms Alzheimer's disease. There's a lot of |
|
|
09:06 | as disorientation and space, as disorientation time disorientation and space. If you |
|
|
09:11 | remember people's names, you don't know you found your way into this |
|
|
09:16 | So you're gonna forget how you found way. So every time you're gonna |
|
|
09:19 | to maybe get help or find your here. So that's disorientation, spatial |
|
|
09:26 | , memory also not remembering spatially where located. What happens if you don't |
|
|
09:31 | things? What happens if you don't keys for five minutes, minutes, |
|
|
09:37 | get mad, you get frustrated, get very anxious and what if you |
|
|
09:41 | find it for two days? You what if it is a common occurrence |
|
|
09:46 | whatever happened, you don't know where put almost anything and you don't remember |
|
|
09:50 | to find it or where it's It causes a lot of anxiety for |
|
|
09:56 | . So there's a lot more to symptomology and it is a terminal disease |
|
|
10:02 | at first, it's the inability of brain to remember things and ability of |
|
|
10:07 | brain to orient themselves, other psychological . And later, as is shown |
|
|
10:15 | , this is a progression of Alzheimer's with a severe stage of Alzheimer's |
|
|
10:23 | which essentially shows significant loss of brain . So you have neurodegeneration, neurons |
|
|
10:32 | degenerating, they're dying. And in , you can see significant loss of |
|
|
10:37 | gray matter in severe cases of Alzheimer's patients. So what happens in this |
|
|
10:45 | ? That means that your brain is not capable of performing all of other |
|
|
10:50 | functions and some of them are including functions. So, heart rate, |
|
|
10:58 | , breathing correctly and that causes the, the terminal and in Alzheimer's |
|
|
11:06 | . Um but if you look at pathological hall hallmarks, the pathology of |
|
|
11:12 | disease, we're gonna talk about two that are happening in and around the |
|
|
11:17 | . The first hallmark of the pathology Alzheimer's disease. The formation of what |
|
|
11:23 | call beta amyloid plaques or amyloid plaques senile plaques, you will hear or |
|
|
11:30 | plaques. Sometimes those are the aggregates the inappropriately cleaved protein that starts aggregating |
|
|
11:41 | of the cells. But as it aggregating and becoming larger and larger and |
|
|
11:48 | in other elements, calcification and causing like a ball of inflammation around that |
|
|
11:56 | . It physically starts infringing on the of neurons. It also has other |
|
|
12:04 | like I said within this chemically kind a inflammatory area that starts affecting neurons |
|
|
12:11 | in particular axons and axonal communication can impaired. So these are extracellular |
|
|
12:20 | Another typical pathology uh Hallmark of Alzheimer's of the formation of neurofibrillary tangles. |
|
|
12:29 | you have tau proteins that essentially on , they accumulate, there's overproduction over |
|
|
12:39 | of the K prodi it accumulates around selma and it causes these tangles. |
|
|
12:45 | so remember we talked about how the skeletal elements are very important for |
|
|
12:53 | So in the case of the what it essentially does, it destroys |
|
|
12:59 | normal structure of the cyto skalpel elements impedes with normal transport. Therefore, |
|
|
13:05 | cannot be transported in and out of SOMA. And the first case when |
|
|
13:11 | starts affecting the the cells from the , the amyloid plaques on the on |
|
|
13:17 | right here and they will start affecting axons. Axons will start failing to |
|
|
13:23 | action potentials. So on the the tangles impair the transport on the |
|
|
13:30 | , they start impairing axonal function and transmission, synoptic communication between neurons. |
|
|
13:40 | . So we'll come back and talk Alzheimer's disease again. Uh Currently I |
|
|
13:47 | there is no medication when we talk acetylcholine signaling, uh immune systems, |
|
|
13:56 | will talk about uh major Alzheimer's medications are based on acetylcholine signaling in the |
|
|
14:04 | . Yeah. Which one is the PP. Yeah. So there's |
|
|
14:14 | there's already amyloid precursor protein. It's a part of our natural brain protein |
|
|
14:22 | . It just gets cleaved inappropriately and it gets cleaved inappropriately, it produces |
|
|
14:28 | aggregates outside outside the cells. So will come back and talk about therapy |
|
|
14:36 | for Alzheimer's disease because the only medications are available right now, they can |
|
|
14:40 | down the progression of this disease. fact, open your news yesterday and |
|
|
14:45 | there is a discussion of potentially two medications coming online experimental for Alzheimer's disease |
|
|
14:52 | show the highest degree of slowing down the progression of the disease and there's |
|
|
14:58 | cure. It's just how well can control the progression of the disease from |
|
|
15:02 | few plaques to thousands of plaques from damage to the brain to severe damage |
|
|
15:09 | the brain structures? As is shown on a uh gross anatomical scale. |
|
|
15:14 | very common comorbidity to Alzheimer's disease is . And what is comorbidity is that |
|
|
15:21 | is epilepsy? It's a traumatic brain . What is comorbidity? What does |
|
|
15:27 | mean? So you already have one that is going to shorten your |
|
|
15:33 | especially if you have a terminal disease Alzheimer's disease, it can shorten your |
|
|
15:38 | uh fairly significantly. And there are things that are happening because of the |
|
|
15:45 | pathology already mentioned, impaired external impaired synaptic transmission, uh which leads |
|
|
15:56 | many different problems which also has inflammation talked about. And maybe these are |
|
|
16:03 | of the common mechanisms that we know between the diseases that allow one disease |
|
|
16:11 | Alzheimer's disease due to the causes of inflammation to call upon sort of a |
|
|
16:19 | . A second disease, allow for second disease to express itself. And |
|
|
16:27 | you, in addition to Alzheimer's patients end up having epilepsy also and having |
|
|
16:35 | . And the second disease is on own, also going to shorten their |
|
|
16:41 | . But now you have two diseases are together co are shortening your lifespan |
|
|
16:51 | than right. Yeah. You just the, don't be the cause. |
|
|
17:02 | . Is it because they stop neuron and then not buy you them or |
|
|
17:09 | something else? Ok. So uh not only neuronal communication but it's pretty |
|
|
17:17 | complex processes that eventually leads to both and necrosis of neurons. But once |
|
|
17:26 | uh die or their axons degenerate in CNS, there's no way to regenerate |
|
|
17:33 | the periphery. If you have a nerve, there is a way to |
|
|
17:39 | and regrow that nerve, if not full, at least in a partial |
|
|
17:44 | of that nerve. But in the , there is no regeneration, there |
|
|
17:50 | some stem cells in adult brains, to a very small extent. And |
|
|
17:57 | e especially at older ages, like Alzheimer's disease, 55. And |
|
|
18:04 | there are a lot of mechanisms that allow for the brain to repair itself |
|
|
18:10 | easily, including a lesser number of progenitor cells, potentially uh stem |
|
|
18:16 | um an inability to control inflammation. inflammation sets itself up, it also |
|
|
18:25 | blood brain barrier, which hopefully we'll about today, which introduces other significant |
|
|
18:32 | . So it it it depends whenever injury in general, the best time |
|
|
18:38 | recover from any injury is in the childhood or early adult years. And |
|
|
18:45 | is from physical injury to the bones muscles uh to uh to a |
|
|
18:53 | for example, like a brain surgery you are not necessarily going to have |
|
|
18:57 | significant loss of function if you're a . But if that same amount of |
|
|
19:03 | brain in that same or similarity gets out in the adult or an aging |
|
|
19:11 | , there's potentially going to be a more of that function of lots, |
|
|
19:15 | lot less of the recovery and So we just are not as well |
|
|
19:21 | in older age to repair our bodies brains. So, you know, |
|
|
19:29 | there's this whole thing of uh really old people trying to uh stay as |
|
|
19:35 | as possible. So you'll see it Tik Tok probably too. You know |
|
|
19:39 | some 65 year old telling you I a body of a 45 year |
|
|
19:45 | like somebody should do a series of have a brain of a right. |
|
|
19:53 | uh brain health is somewhat related to too. So having a healthy body |
|
|
19:59 | important because a lot of that energy exercise actually uh benefits our brains as |
|
|
20:06 | . The lactate function. OK. move on to dendrites and dendrites. |
|
|
20:12 | already discussed the pretty complex. They the apical dendrites at the apex of |
|
|
20:18 | we typically consider an excitatory petal c is apical dendrites and then you have |
|
|
20:24 | dendrites and a lot of these dendrites have dendritic spines, they will be |
|
|
20:30 | , but there's also smooth dendrites and will not contain spines. So it's |
|
|
20:35 | necessarily that all of the dendrites will dendritic spines. Now, if you |
|
|
20:42 | trying to classify neurons from the very times of Ramonica reconstructions of all of |
|
|
20:50 | cells, you already started sorting through classes of neurons based on their |
|
|
20:56 | So when you're exposed to the cells the go stain, you have their |
|
|
21:00 | morphology, you can start comparing cells are similar morphological and see if you |
|
|
21:06 | find the same or similar cells in parts of the brain. So from |
|
|
21:11 | point on, we became interested in neurons and understanding how many different types |
|
|
21:18 | neurons there are what are some of features that will qualify one neuron to |
|
|
21:24 | a subtype A for example, versus D or E or so on and |
|
|
21:29 | forth. So, neuronal morphology, you can see here, these are |
|
|
21:34 | your typical parameter cells with these What's interesting about the dendritic spines? |
|
|
21:41 | already discussed this. But what's really about the spines is that if you |
|
|
21:46 | , they have the mitochondria, they smooth and the plastic reticulum, they |
|
|
21:56 | synaptic polyribosome complexes in the spines which them to be somewhat biochemically independent to |
|
|
22:08 | a certain degree and the ability to post translational modifications right there locally at |
|
|
22:15 | level of the spine, not necessarily the rest of the cell and the |
|
|
22:20 | to a limited degree and a lot energy is being consumed again because on |
|
|
22:26 | sides, on the presynaptic side where have the red pre synoptic terminals and |
|
|
22:32 | red vesicles there, you'll have a of A TP for release and recycling |
|
|
22:37 | the vesicles. And on the po side equally, you'll have a need |
|
|
22:43 | lot of a TP, you'll have there. Now, these spines are |
|
|
22:49 | about one micrometer in size and they the most dynamic units in the |
|
|
22:59 | And by dynamic, I mean is their functionality can change, they can |
|
|
23:04 | become stronger, their shape can become . So you can reorganize cyto skeletal |
|
|
23:11 | underneath make the synopsis or dendritic spines . Now, they can contain two |
|
|
23:20 | three po synoptic densities versus just one synoptic density here. For example, |
|
|
23:26 | have one pos synaptic density, 23 densities because this mushroom like shade spine |
|
|
23:36 | for a lot of surface area. a powerful synapse there. And you |
|
|
23:42 | also reshape and make those synapses and smaller. And when you make them |
|
|
23:49 | , they may just contain one pos density. Therefore, they're not going |
|
|
23:54 | have as much impact on the dendrite the whole pos synoptic neuron. So |
|
|
24:02 | strengthening or weakening of synaptic communication because the synapse is larger, has more |
|
|
24:09 | synoptic densities is receiving strong input, going to be re replying or responding |
|
|
24:15 | that input more reliably. If it's smaller input or smaller processing capability is |
|
|
24:23 | to be responding to that to a degree. So, strengthening or weakening |
|
|
24:29 | a part of what we call neuronal . In some instances, the spines |
|
|
24:37 | not just reshaped, they're not just and malleable, they're not just |
|
|
24:42 | but they can be completely driven away new spines can be created. So |
|
|
24:50 | is what we call activity dependent neuronal or activity dependent synaptic plasticity because the |
|
|
24:59 | is happening at the synopsis where two are communicating with each other presynaptic and |
|
|
25:06 | . And although we're talking mostly about dendritic spines of postsynaptic changes that can |
|
|
25:13 | equally. So the pre synoptic terminals also be more effective and larger in |
|
|
25:20 | versus smaller and less effective or not and causing the release of neurotransmitter at |
|
|
25:26 | for one reason or another. In , when we're born, everything is |
|
|
25:31 | lot more interconnected in our brains than is when we refine those connections through |
|
|
25:37 | growth process, through activity dependent process through all of the external stimuli that |
|
|
25:43 | getting as part of our early in particular postnatal development. So the |
|
|
25:50 | number decreases and the connections we find . If everything is interconnected. At |
|
|
25:56 | and later, there's only two or specific connections to develop in these more |
|
|
26:02 | , more developed neuronal macs. But spine number can change and synoptic number |
|
|
26:11 | number of synopses between one neuron to other. One neuron can have 10 |
|
|
26:16 | onto the other neuron. It can more synapses have 15 soy naps |
|
|
26:21 | That means it's gonna have way more on this post synaptic neuron. And |
|
|
26:27 | so from the stem synopsis, it go down a number to 6754321 and |
|
|
26:33 | completely eliminate the connection with this And that happens because there is not |
|
|
26:38 | stimulation, there's not enough activity that this neuron or there is not enough |
|
|
26:44 | from this neuron, right? How can you talk to somebody and they |
|
|
26:48 | respond to you before you stop talking them? Pounds, right? So |
|
|
26:55 | will reshape spine level will reshape and of these things. A number of |
|
|
27:00 | and locations, the density of spines the shaft are very, very |
|
|
27:06 | It can be controlled by activity and by genes. And what happens if |
|
|
27:14 | have abnormal formations of the spine. here we're going to look at this |
|
|
27:19 | already third neurological disorder. So, about epilepsy today, Alzheimer's disease and |
|
|
27:27 | Fragile X syndrome, it's a syndrome you have single gene expression and missing |
|
|
27:36 | which is on X chromosome. So affects boys more and the X chromosome |
|
|
27:42 | fragile. Uh And what it does this is a early developmental disorder. |
|
|
27:51 | an intellectual disability uh with certain phenotypic . So that's, that's another interesting |
|
|
28:00 | for you to start thinking about. , first of all, there's |
|
|
28:05 | right? Symptomologies, I'm forgetting But if one individual, two older |
|
|
28:11 | or ladies walked in, they were 75 years of age and one of |
|
|
28:17 | was forgetting things and another one was , will, will you be able |
|
|
28:22 | tell that the ones that are They look a certain way, they |
|
|
28:25 | a certain phenotype, they have giant or something like that. No. |
|
|
28:30 | this is on the outside now. in certain neurological disorders, you will |
|
|
28:36 | phenotypic outward features and fragile X is of them. So a lot of |
|
|
28:42 | patients with that fragile x will have face shape and fairly large ears. |
|
|
28:51 | so obviously, this is not how neurologist would diagnose somebody with fragile |
|
|
28:56 | but this is just one of the of reference from them. Symptomology. |
|
|
29:02 | , if there is a phenotypic feature adds on, is there a genetic |
|
|
29:08 | because there's a genetic component to finally come up with a, with |
|
|
29:13 | with a diagnosis. It's a complex . It falls under autism spectrum disorders |
|
|
29:20 | it's essentially under autism spectrum disorders are considered a comorbidity because autism spectrum disorders |
|
|
29:29 | a very large umbrella uh disorders that a genetic component that sometimes don't have |
|
|
29:36 | component, behavioral component, uh it has to do with learning and |
|
|
29:41 | features. But obviously, this is and that's why sometimes it's a |
|
|
29:48 | Sometimes it's under the umbrella. That's interpretation of fraudulent acts. Now, |
|
|
29:54 | of the things that's important that relates what we're studying is this is a |
|
|
30:00 | spine from an input that has intellectual . And what was noticed is that |
|
|
30:08 | spines are very different. And it's not all about how long they |
|
|
30:14 | or how big they are. It's about the variety of the shapes that |
|
|
30:18 | come into their locations along the the densities where they're located in higher |
|
|
30:26 | versus smaller densities. And this is of the hallmarks, one of the |
|
|
30:34 | also for fragile X, it is and the spa. Now, if |
|
|
30:40 | look at this image here, what shows is that in green everywhere where |
|
|
30:47 | see a green God, we call a green punkt. You have glutamate |
|
|
30:53 | , you have an excitatory synapse and shown as glue R stands for glutamate |
|
|
31:01 | , those are excitatory receptors and these be excitatory synopsis on that c but |
|
|
31:08 | same cell also has all of these dots on it and these orange dots |
|
|
31:14 | plant are for Gaba receptors and Gaba . And Gaba synopsis are the major |
|
|
31:24 | synopsis in this neuron and also in brain. And what this illustrates is |
|
|
31:31 | single neuron will contain thousands, sometimes of thousands, sometimes even hundreds of |
|
|
31:39 | of synapses. A lot of them going to be excitatory. That means |
|
|
31:43 | there is excitatory inputs projecting onto the . But a lot of them are |
|
|
31:49 | to be inhibitory, which there's going be a lot of inhibitory inputs projecting |
|
|
31:56 | the cell. And the job of neuron is to take all of the |
|
|
32:02 | information. And sometimes you may have synopsis being activated at the same |
|
|
32:08 | In different ratios, there might be excitatory synopsis and 400 inhibitory in all |
|
|
32:16 | of different combinations. But so now happens if you impair the sri |
|
|
32:23 | if you impair the indri spines, if you impair the communication, that |
|
|
32:28 | you cannot process properly all of the or inhibitory inputs onto the south. |
|
|
32:35 | , again, the communication for of the interpretation potentially of the external stimuli |
|
|
32:41 | the communication between the south is going be different than these individuals. So |
|
|
32:49 | get back to classifying neurons. We to understand how neurons are. And |
|
|
32:54 | one of the simple ways again is look at their morphology, unipolar |
|
|
33:00 | bipolar cell and a multipolar cell. which case, multipolar cell has projections |
|
|
33:08 | in all directions and all the poles the bipolar cell goes north south and |
|
|
33:14 | , just sort of has one process bifurcates. So what are some of |
|
|
33:20 | cells? Morphologically uh unipolar cell would an invertebrate neuron, bipolar cell would |
|
|
33:29 | a sensory uh cell that will study the visual system called the bipolar cell |
|
|
33:35 | retina. When we look at the circuit uh pseudo unipolar cell. Oh |
|
|
33:41 | unipolar cell is a ganglion cell of root. Does that ring a |
|
|
33:49 | Remember we talked about how you have cord? So like a butterfly like |
|
|
33:57 | and this is the dorsal side and is the ventral side here. And |
|
|
34:07 | we have this bundle here and this where the SOMA are of the dorsal |
|
|
34:14 | cells, right? And they have axons going into the periphery. |
|
|
34:21 | So this is the peripheral axon and they have another axon is called the |
|
|
34:26 | axon. OK. And that goes the spinal cord. And then you |
|
|
34:31 | study that the synapse onto the cells . And from the natural side, |
|
|
34:36 | have multipolar cells. So this is unipolar dorsal root ganglion cell. Is |
|
|
34:44 | excitatory or inhibitory? It's excitatory. understand what that means. And then |
|
|
34:57 | neuron and motor neuron goes and becomes part of the same spinal nerve, |
|
|
35:05 | . You have spinal nerves here and is the motor output. Is it |
|
|
35:12 | inhibitory? It's only excited part and uses the C toline. That's something |
|
|
35:23 | you should have studied in the anatomy physiology. So, neuromuscular junction, |
|
|
35:29 | ? We'll talk about spinal cord OK. So we have dorsal gang |
|
|
35:34 | cells and this is something that's an thing. I usually tell uh my |
|
|
35:41 | that I will have questions about different class. There are certain things that |
|
|
35:49 | want to know about the set So for example, we already talked |
|
|
35:55 | the Raid all styles or SORU And so uh say we're gonna talk |
|
|
36:09 | motor neuron. What are some of things that we want to know about |
|
|
36:18 | style? So for example, I know, are they excited for, |
|
|
36:22 | the inventory? What is neurotransmitter that release when lo the world more would |
|
|
36:33 | out? What is their morphology? are great uh midterm one matching |
|
|
36:45 | And we'll learn about these cells over next uh uh couple of lectures, |
|
|
36:51 | and more details, but it's good start inquiring and understanding what those cells |
|
|
36:57 | and what they are. So a of rumors, for example, for |
|
|
37:01 | transit will release a see of the brown cells which are called to |
|
|
37:08 | lina well as gang against them. you should start making yourself like a |
|
|
37:15 | table like that because that is going ask you like for example, which |
|
|
37:20 | are motor neurons located on ventr or . And this type of note taking |
|
|
37:26 | general and even taking a sound or it, apical basal no rides |
|
|
37:32 | things like that. Even some basic sometimes can be very helpful. So |
|
|
37:36 | lot of the cells in the brain multipolar cells. The motor neuron is |
|
|
37:41 | multipolar cell. The theme cell is cell. This is a fini cell |
|
|
37:48 | the cells is a multipolar cell and contain all sorts of different shapes and |
|
|
37:55 | bifurcations in anatomies. If you can the spinal motor neuron, on average |
|
|
38:00 | contain about 10,000 synopsis. But a uh the tree containing cell like cell |
|
|
38:09 | contain 100 50,000 synopsis. So that's amount of information that a single |
|
|
38:15 | a single unit has to process. how else do we classify neurons? |
|
|
38:24 | , projection cells verses into neurons. projection cells versus into neurons. What |
|
|
38:30 | projection cells? Projection cells are the that are typically when we talk about |
|
|
38:38 | cells, we're gonna talk about petal . That means they are, they |
|
|
38:43 | excited cells release glutamate. That means project from one neural network or one |
|
|
38:52 | of the brain into the other area the brain. As opposed to inter |
|
|
39:00 | , interneurons are the cells that will located here in this neural network. |
|
|
39:07 | these interneurons typically are inhibitory and typically release Gaba as the major neurotransmitter and |
|
|
39:18 | can communicate to each other. They communicate to parameter cells but they will |
|
|
39:26 | have axons that project long distances into other structures. OK. So these |
|
|
39:32 | projection cells typically parameter cells and these interneurons. So that's another way in |
|
|
39:48 | we can classify the bad excitability, cells release glutamate inhibitory cells release |
|
|
40:02 | But is that all the chemicals? , we'll study many, well, |
|
|
40:08 | more chemicals in the brain at And uh it gets a little bit |
|
|
40:13 | complex than just excited or inhibitory neuropeptides. So what type of uh |
|
|
40:23 | they are? The projection cell, kind of neurotransmitter they release? What |
|
|
40:29 | of neuropeptides or cells? Specific They may contain cells, specific markers |
|
|
40:35 | call they may contain. This is first published in intercellular action potential |
|
|
40:44 | So in 19 thirties and especially during war two and after world war |
|
|
40:51 | the development of pretty sophisticated electrical circuits especially for the naval ships and the |
|
|
41:01 | uh for the navy and these oscilloscopes are really fast, they also developed |
|
|
41:07 | scientists start using these oscilloscopes in the thirties and forties later that they become |
|
|
41:14 | oscilloscopes and capturing really fast activity on fast activity is the action potential. |
|
|
41:20 | neurons produce action potentials that are about to 23 milliseconds in duration and about |
|
|
41:29 | millivolts in amplitude. It's a very electrical potential in neurons, action |
|
|
41:37 | And so if before 19 forties, , we understood the shapes of the |
|
|
41:45 | because we had all of these dyes we have microscopes, then it was |
|
|
41:51 | for us to understand the activity of cells. Can we correlate certain |
|
|
41:57 | Do petal cells have a certain what we call pattern of action |
|
|
42:04 | So, firing signatures. So action is like firing because it's so |
|
|
42:10 | Uh do they thermal cells have different potential patterns from the interneurons? Or |
|
|
42:19 | they all the same across the So you can be a parameter cell |
|
|
42:24 | inter neuron, dorsal ganglion cell or neuron. And you will always produce |
|
|
42:29 | same action for Schultz and the same . And with these experiments, we |
|
|
42:36 | start unraveling the different cell subs have patterns of actual potentials. And that |
|
|
42:42 | can indeed correlate these patterns to specific uh genetic expression. Once we have |
|
|
42:53 | tools to analyze cells genetically, now can start and generating antibodies, for |
|
|
43:02 | . And we're talking about the second of the 20th century, you're generating |
|
|
43:06 | , you're tagging cells, you're doing to chemistry, you're revealing different |
|
|
43:11 | different neurotransmitters across the brain and also . Once you have the genetic |
|
|
43:18 | you can study what unique subsets of that are on these cells that are |
|
|
43:26 | what unique proteins or receptors that are in these cells. And we realize |
|
|
43:33 | these subtypes of cells are different because express slightly different subset of genes. |
|
|
43:39 | there might be 100 different subtypes of , they all neurons, but they |
|
|
43:45 | have a variation in the subset of genes that they express. And the |
|
|
43:50 | and molecules that they carry. Although have the same code, like every |
|
|
43:56 | has the same code DNA, but every cell is going to utilize all |
|
|
44:04 | the time, the same pieces of code. And that's what makes cells |
|
|
44:09 | by expressing that code differently, having abilities to uh to differ morphologically and |
|
|
44:18 | . OK. Now we are doing like single cell RN A sequences and |
|
|
44:24 | throughput. How many 100,000 cells a where we can analyze uh all of |
|
|
44:31 | trusts it's called can't pronounce it trans pretty cool stuff. So now we |
|
|
44:43 | that neurons and we can visualize them infrared microscopy. We can put electrodes |
|
|
44:49 | we find two cells here. This for my reporting at the beginning of |
|
|
44:56 | 21st century. Uh So you have cells and they have two micro electrodes |
|
|
45:02 | them and we give them the same stimulus. So the cell on the |
|
|
45:07 | and the cell on the right, see the exact same stimulus from that |
|
|
45:12 | . And what you see there, one of these lines, every one |
|
|
45:15 | these six is an action potential. these are action potentials. So each |
|
|
45:28 | of these sticks is like an Each one uses a natural. And |
|
|
45:43 | can see that the cell on the once you give it more and more |
|
|
45:48 | the stimulus which would really inject more more positive current, it responds with |
|
|
45:54 | very fast pattern, very fast frequency action potentials and it sustains that very |
|
|
46:01 | frequency. And the cell on the , it gets the same input from |
|
|
46:06 | electrode. So the same stimulus or of the same stimulus, but it |
|
|
46:11 | with a much slower pattern of action . And also that spaces itself what |
|
|
46:18 | call accommodates it goes from faster frequencies slower frequencies during the response of the |
|
|
46:26 | . So we understood pretty clearly that cell subtypes will differ functionally. Both |
|
|
46:38 | these cells speak neuronal language of action . But I call this a different |
|
|
46:46 | . So the cell on the left different dialect from the cell on the |
|
|
46:52 | , this is a functional output of cell action potential is going to cause |
|
|
46:57 | release of the neurotransmitter. That's why functional output of the cell that eventually |
|
|
47:03 | cause a certain pattern of communication. certain pattern of neurotransmitter release very fast |
|
|
47:09 | sustained pattern from one cell versus very and less sustained pattern from another |
|
|
47:18 | So it all influences neuronal communication of direct these recording. So we can |
|
|
47:24 | really smart and we can place a that is called neuro, sometimes also |
|
|
47:29 | biocyte and neuro and dye once it's inside the cell. So during the |
|
|
47:37 | , you have an electrode and it have this neuro and dye and this |
|
|
47:42 | will enter into the cell and it expose all of its beautiful morphology. |
|
|
47:49 | as we do these recordings, we the cells using infrared microscopy. There's |
|
|
47:54 | stain, we give them electrical stimulus re record their functional electrical response or |
|
|
48:02 | documents. Then during the recording, fill the sauce with the dye after |
|
|
48:10 | recording, we process the dyes and reconstruct the precise morphology of these cells |
|
|
48:21 | finally, we can crossing them. , apart from this dye after or |
|
|
48:28 | the process of processing and then revealing dye in the tissue concurrently before or |
|
|
48:34 | there's different ways. But you can use antibodies to see if those cells |
|
|
48:43 | specific markers. So immuno chemistry and we do all of this, we |
|
|
48:52 | the morphology, we know the We know the location of the |
|
|
48:57 | what network they're located, we know markers they stay for intercellular. Then |
|
|
49:03 | really good at refining and defining that cell subtype. And of course, |
|
|
49:12 | you have the ability to suck up cytoplasm inject the dye but also suck |
|
|
49:18 | the cytoplasm. You could do single RN A analysis on that also. |
|
|
49:25 | this is being now replaced by some the high throughput uh RN A sequences |
|
|
49:31 | are available. This is an example a patch of a cortex and then |
|
|
49:39 | patch of the cortex. This is small patch of the cortex. Number |
|
|
49:43 | single cells, about 10 micrometers in . Many of the cells were |
|
|
49:49 | We called it patch. The cell a microelectrode were recorded from different cells |
|
|
49:55 | this um cortical circuit here and all these cells received an identical input and |
|
|
50:04 | can see that certain cells responded again these fast sustained sequences of action potentials |
|
|
50:14 | . So, although these cells here receiving exact same input, but they |
|
|
50:20 | respond, all barns will go the on top beat called stuttering cells because |
|
|
50:35 | can give them a continuous input, stimulus and they will stutter. So |
|
|
50:41 | will produce these stuttered trains of action that cells like it's not continuous as |
|
|
50:49 | would see here. And so now I demonstrated it, I want you |
|
|
50:55 | to pick your favorite um run it just for a good vocal exercise. |
|
|
51:02 | on. You don't have to be . It's just like boom, |
|
|
51:10 | boom, boom, boom. At point, I have a TED |
|
|
51:15 | I I should have done this. I said it's like because that's what |
|
|
51:20 | potential sound like when you actually can them from electrical signals into an |
|
|
51:27 | But I like this illustration. So but but you're getting the um the |
|
|
51:34 | flow here in the sense that these all different dialects. And now you |
|
|
51:41 | expect that because these cells speak different . What would you expect? There's |
|
|
51:46 | to be different about those cells, morphology, there's cellular gene expression, |
|
|
51:54 | , certain proteins and channels that are be different? OK. What else |
|
|
52:01 | neurotransmitters that they release may be different they are projection cells versus interneurons? |
|
|
52:11 | . So there's many different methods and gold standard of defining a neuronal |
|
|
52:20 | right? There could be a There's 100 no there's 100 40 |
|
|
52:23 | there's 100 4347 and so on and forth. You could have this |
|
|
52:29 | the moving target for the gold standard the subs of the cells. It |
|
|
52:32 | moving as the technologies are improving. , but we can do our best |
|
|
52:37 | trying to uh very clearly define them much as we can using as many |
|
|
52:43 | tools as we have to or as as we have available or have |
|
|
52:48 | Or this was my setup at George University where I did my second postdoctoral |
|
|
52:55 | which was on epilepsy. And in , in my study, I was |
|
|
53:00 | to know what cells start, epileptic , which subtype of cells. So |
|
|
53:07 | wasn't enough for me to know Oh, this cell started, I |
|
|
53:11 | to know what subtype of the cell it that started these seizures. And |
|
|
53:15 | answer that question. And to this , actually, if you want to |
|
|
53:20 | to the resolution specificity on electrical activity individual cells, you still have to |
|
|
53:26 | the single cell patch lamp or electrophysiological . And if you wanted to know |
|
|
53:34 | one starts, which cell starts you need at least two cells to |
|
|
53:39 | that. And it is better if have three cells and it is best |
|
|
53:43 | you have four cells, you can from concurrently. If you're mad |
|
|
53:48 | I think world record is seven and is somewhat automated at the same |
|
|
53:52 | very difficult long experiments. But they us some very interesting activity in the |
|
|
53:59 | tissue that we published. And two three years later, the same type |
|
|
54:06 | activity was discovered in human living human . So what you find in the |
|
|
54:12 | is another lesson. It's not only said, well, Goji just |
|
|
54:16 | I'm just gonna bring the stain and it on the brains. You can |
|
|
54:19 | a lot of innovation, academic so to speak. But uh also |
|
|
54:26 | you find in the lab. A of times it's like, what does |
|
|
54:29 | have to do with me with my ? Well, a lot and a |
|
|
54:37 | of things that you find at first simpler models in simpler organisms. We |
|
|
54:44 | a lot of genetic similarity. We homology of proteins with many different species |
|
|
54:51 | this world. We can learn a of things and from these types of |
|
|
54:57 | , we learned that the inhibitory cells the ones that start seizures. It |
|
|
55:02 | very surprising and it took us about years to prove to the scientific community |
|
|
55:06 | reviewers that that is the case. after we did it then just a |
|
|
55:11 | later, there was a human study living brains showing very similar patterns of |
|
|
55:17 | from inhibitory neurons starting seizures as So it's really, really gratifying um |
|
|
55:24 | , to know that your work can an impact uh and be applicable |
|
|
55:30 | to society classifying neurons. This is example of a really famous circuit really |
|
|
55:36 | started circuit in the brain that we the hippocampus. OK. And the |
|
|
55:42 | mainly consists of the three layers. the top layer is stratum radium. |
|
|
55:49 | have it labeled here on the la , then the white stratum ravalli bottom |
|
|
55:57 | ST or so for the most they count all structures of three layered |
|
|
56:03 | . And what is portrayed here confuses uh a lot of students and uh |
|
|
56:12 | asked me two or three times, do they need to know for the |
|
|
56:16 | ? But this is a publication from . So when you talk about something |
|
|
56:22 | is still relevant, this is still relevant. Of course, there's |
|
|
56:27 | refinements and improvements. Single cell RN uh high through blah blah blah. |
|
|
56:32 | molecular analysis have been refined and played the subtyping of the cells. But |
|
|
56:37 | still illustrates how one really can know many subtypes of cells there are. |
|
|
56:44 | this is the hippocampal circuit. And the circuit charges show these cells in |
|
|
56:49 | blue and these are the parameter cells are projection cells that means that they're |
|
|
56:56 | with the axons are going to exit of the cans. Communicate that information |
|
|
57:01 | the other platforms. Exci cells to glu they mostly live in strata from |
|
|
57:10 | , some of them live in the two layers. Morphologically, the subtypes |
|
|
57:15 | the cells the three that are shown identical. So the location of the |
|
|
57:21 | where you find their CS in Galla Ra Island is a little different. |
|
|
57:26 | else is different? The only thing CD which stands for called be turns |
|
|
57:31 | that some parameter projection cells express cal and others do not. And this |
|
|
57:39 | fairly simple for the excitatory projection cells the hippocampus. That means those cells |
|
|
57:46 | super important for communicating information to the networks uh of the brain. |
|
|
57:54 | you see surrounding these petal solace, , all the way through 21. |
|
|
58:01 | are inhibitory interneurons that are staying in circuit locally. They're inhibitory because they |
|
|
58:10 | Gaba, they're interneurons because they do have axons that leave the hippocampus and |
|
|
58:18 | to other parts of the brain. about 80 to 90% of all the |
|
|
58:25 | in the hippocampus are excited or petal . So if you look at this |
|
|
58:34 | that and hippocampus, 80 to 90% the cells are pinal cells which leads |
|
|
58:45 | to 20% of all the cells as neurons. And there isn't that much |
|
|
58:52 | variety. And as far as the , despite the fact that interneurons only |
|
|
59:00 | for up to 20% of all of cells of the campus, there's at |
|
|
59:05 | 21 different subtypes of those interneurons. there is a lot more complexity in |
|
|
59:13 | network processing, especially local network processing stems from the inhibitory cells. Those |
|
|
59:24 | , some of them live in others live in red is the orange |
|
|
59:30 | red. We have dendrites, some their dendrites will be extending vertically. |
|
|
59:36 | will be extending horizontally, those yellow and those purple processes. These are |
|
|
59:44 | and the yellow cuffs of synopsis. it shows that certain neurons will form |
|
|
59:49 | right on the c of the projection and other into neurons will target the |
|
|
59:56 | dendrites of the parameter cells and yet will target the basal dendrites and even |
|
|
60:02 | axons of the parameter cells. So about this network pretty complex where you |
|
|
60:10 | 21 different players. These players will different dialects, ta ta, |
|
|
60:16 | ta, ta, ta, ta bla bla bla bla bla, |
|
|
60:17 | ? They're all speaking with each other this network. They're speaking to the |
|
|
60:22 | cells, they're controlling parameter cells at apex on the SOMA on the base |
|
|
60:30 | on the output to this inhibitory neurons a lot of control of the petal |
|
|
60:38 | and they will determine what kind of gets projected into the adjacent network. |
|
|
60:45 | can actually completely because they inhibit they can completely quench and suppress any |
|
|
60:51 | and nothing is going to project from A to network B. They can |
|
|
60:58 | a certain pattern, a certain dialect will dominate, which will influence this |
|
|
61:04 | cell to communicate through this network in specific pattern. Allowed I communicate |
|
|
61:10 | I don't communicate. So you have level of complexity at the local circuit |
|
|
61:18 | that is introduced by the inhibitory There are much more diversity in the |
|
|
61:25 | cells morphologically synaptic. And also if finally do this cellular analysis of molecular |
|
|
61:33 | of cell specific markers, it's not you to memorize this. But some |
|
|
61:38 | the in your cells like two and look identical. So what's the difference |
|
|
61:43 | two and four? They may even the same dialect. But two is |
|
|
61:48 | basket cell that expresses pro provide and is this basket cell that expresses another |
|
|
61:55 | to kind of a different marking side it which qualifies it as a different |
|
|
62:01 | of the cell given other uh features things that go to the analysis. |
|
|
62:07 | what do you need to know for exam about this is that in the |
|
|
62:12 | , which is predominantly three layered you have a lot of inhibitory |
|
|
62:16 | Uh Now how you tell the difference inhibitor excitatory cells know that the exciter |
|
|
62:21 | are projection cells, inhibitory cells are because they release gaba the local circuit |
|
|
62:28 | . But there are also a lot variety of these cells. So they |
|
|
62:33 | really impact and determine what kind of gets communicated out of that network into |
|
|
62:38 | adjacent and contribute a lot to allowing the adjacent networks to communicate and synchronize |
|
|
62:46 | or not communicate at all and get of sync, right? And that's |
|
|
62:55 | important for neuronal plasticity and learning and . So, Ramon Kahal, when |
|
|
63:04 | was drawing different subtypes of cells, can see he drew around, he |
|
|
63:08 | drawing with PC, he also glu lose glia. So he also stained |
|
|
63:20 | in human cerebella. So on the in B are different types of Astros |
|
|
63:27 | the time, he named them as glia smooth for the plasm with as |
|
|
63:34 | for V Astros that have be marketing with an app pretty remarkable, |
|
|
63:42 | So he already sept died, not neurons based on their morphology because he |
|
|
63:47 | record action potentials. He did not immunochemistry available to him or a high |
|
|
63:55 | with RN a sequence, right? But he also started defining morphologies of |
|
|
64:02 | glial cells. And in this he was a pioneer in this area |
|
|
64:08 | well. So for glial cells, have four major subtypes of real cells |
|
|
64:14 | we will discuss in the sports radial , oligodendrocytes, microglia and astrocytes. |
|
|
64:22 | glia is very important for the development neuronal processes, neuronal migration and |
|
|
64:29 | What does that mean? Neuronal Where are neurons migrating? Were they |
|
|
64:33 | guided to they're migrating because they are in specific areas of the brain. |
|
|
64:40 | neurons are not born you layer cortical neuron and occipital lobe is not |
|
|
64:45 | in layer six and occipital lobe. born in specific zones around inside your |
|
|
64:54 | . Actually and then from those specific , they migrate into their final destinations |
|
|
65:02 | to migrate to their final destinations, need guides that will get them to |
|
|
65:07 | final destinations. And radial glia have their physical guides to those final destinations |
|
|
65:14 | the neurons. Oligodendrocytes are important for and supportive neurons. When we talk |
|
|
65:22 | insulation, we talk about oligodendrocytes wrapping the axons and forming myelin sheets around |
|
|
65:30 | axons of neurons Microglia is the most mobile elements in the brain that |
|
|
65:38 | involved in injury, cleanup, scavenging an injury and repair and immune |
|
|
65:47 | So, microglia released cytokines and you have heard when COVID-19 was in full |
|
|
65:54 | . Even National Public Radio was talking the cytokine storms. You know what |
|
|
65:59 | side? It's not like, you , gulf storm is different cytokine |
|
|
66:04 | It's like you have microglia that control that release cytokines to call upon the |
|
|
66:11 | response. It's normal response if you an infection, inflammation, injury in |
|
|
66:16 | brain. But if it goes out control, the cytokine release goes out |
|
|
66:21 | control. And you have these cytokine including promoted by microglia, it can |
|
|
66:28 | more damage than a positive response in brain and can perpetuate the process of |
|
|
66:36 | . As Tracy, you can see Asy there and you can see that |
|
|
66:40 | end and processes of the ostracizes of top, right is wrapped around the |
|
|
66:46 | , wrapped around the dendrites and the process form the feet on the |
|
|
66:53 | on the blood vessels. So, is very important in synoptic plasticity and |
|
|
67:01 | release in synaptic formation. We call of synaptic formation, synaptic genesis. |
|
|
67:09 | astrocytes are also very important because there checkpoints. They are one of the |
|
|
67:13 | checkpoints at the blood brain barrier. the barrier between the blood and the |
|
|
67:19 | . So they will patrol and monitor that can enter or not from the |
|
|
67:25 | into the brain. So a lot functions that glia is responsible for. |
|
|
67:31 | I said, you won't have a if you have, don't have the |
|
|
67:35 | and glia will influence neuronal migration growth neuron, synaptic formation, synaptic |
|
|
67:44 | homeostasis of the brain as a as whole, in particular microglia, ostroy |
|
|
67:51 | controlling these processes of inflammation of chemicals even energy and neurotransmitter release, especially |
|
|
68:00 | the excitatory glutamate cells. This is of glial cells that are involved in |
|
|
68:07 | development of neuronal processes. So glial literally have this process here that gets |
|
|
68:17 | by neurons. And what neuron does neuron climes along this process. So |
|
|
68:25 | actually have cytoplasmic continuity between neurons and glial cells. And neuron will use |
|
|
68:34 | radial glial cell like a rope like lattice to migrate and to find its |
|
|
68:40 | destination. Hopefully, this link will . Hopefully there's no commercial in |
|
|
68:57 | This is an example, it's working my screen, but it's it's not |
|
|
69:01 | on your screen. It's not showing of it. Folks are dropping |
|